Electrophysiological correlates of observational learning in children.

Observational learning is an important mechanism for cognitive and social development. However, the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying observational learning in children are not well understood. In this study, we used a probabilistic reward-based observational learning paradigm to compare behavioral and electrophysiological markers of individual and observational reinforcement learning in 8- to 10-year-old children. Specifically, we manipulated the amount of observable information as well as children's similarity in age to the observed person (same-aged child vs. adult) to examine the effects of similarity in age on the integration of observed information in children. We show that the feedback-related negativity (FRN) during individual reinforcement learning reflects the valence of outcomes of own actions. Furthermore, we found that the feedback-related negativity during observational reinforcement learning (oFRN) showed a similar distinction between outcome valences of observed actions. This suggests that the oFRN can serve as a measure of observational learning in middle childhood. Moreover, during observational learning children profited from the additional social information and imitated the choices of their own peers more than those of adults, indicating that children have a tendency to conform more with similar others (e.g. their own peers) compared to dissimilar others (adults). Taken together, our results show that children can benefit from integrating observable information and that oFRN may serve as a measure of observational learning in children.

[1]  H. Schroeder,et al.  Importance of model similarity on extinction of avoidance behavior in children. , 1975, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[2]  Zolinda Stoneman,et al.  Selective Imitation of Same-Age, Older, and Younger Peer Models. , 1981 .

[3]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  T. Gog,et al.  Example-Based Learning: Integrating Cognitive and Social-Cognitive Research Perspectives , 2010 .

[5]  W. Schultz,et al.  Neural mechanisms of observational learning , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Error-Related Negativity Predicts Reinforcement Learning and Conflict Biases , 2005, Neuron.

[7]  R. E. Martin Event-related potential studies of outcome processing and feedback-guided learning , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[8]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Transformations in the Couplings Among Intellectual Abilities and Constituent Cognitive Processes Across the Life Span , 2004, Psychological science.

[9]  Adrian G. Fischer,et al.  Neural mechanisms and temporal dynamics of performance monitoring , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  Jutta Kray,et al.  Developmental changes in performance monitoring: How electrophysiological data can enhance our understanding of error and feedback processing in childhood and adolescence , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Age, sex and individual differences in punishment sensitivity: factors influencing the feedback-related negativity. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[12]  B. Eppinger,et al.  Better or worse than expected? Aging, learning, and the ERN , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  ERP correlates of feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence of response choice. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[15]  N. Zmyj,et al.  The Role of a Model's Age for Young Children's Imitation: A Research Review , 2013 .

[16]  M. Vanderborght,et al.  Who Knows Best? Preschoolers Sometimes Prefer Child Informants Over Adult Informants. , 2009, Infant and child development.

[17]  Carl R. Owens,et al.  Effects of the Model's Age, Perceived Similarity, and Familiarity on Children's Donating , 1991 .

[18]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Striatum-medial prefrontal cortex connectivity predicts developmental changes in reinforcement learning. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[20]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[21]  William J. Gehring,et al.  The Error-Related Negativity (ERN/Ne) , 2011 .

[22]  S. Geisser,et al.  An Extension of Box's Results on the Use of the $F$ Distribution in Multivariate Analysis , 1958 .

[23]  Ellen L. Usher,et al.  Social Cognitive Theory and Motivation , 2012, The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation.

[24]  Rebecca J. Compton,et al.  Perceived similarity and neural mirroring: Evidence from vicarious error processing , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[25]  E. Crone,et al.  The role of the medial frontal cortex in the development of cognitive and social-affective performance monitoring. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[26]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[27]  Emma Flynn,et al.  Context-dependent model-based biases in cultural transmission: children's imitation is affected by model age over model knowledge state , 2012 .

[28]  D. Hämmerer,et al.  Dopaminergic and prefrontal contributions to reward-based learning and outcome monitoring during child development and aging. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[29]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[30]  Jun'ichi Katayama,et al.  Self-relevant criteria determine the evaluation of outcomes induced by others , 2008, Neuroreport.

[31]  Viktor Müller,et al.  Life Span Differences in Electrophysiological Correlates of Monitoring Gains and Losses during Probabilistic Reinforcement Learning , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Learning about causes from people: observational causal learning in 24-month-old infants. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[33]  Andrew N Meltzoff,et al.  Neural correlates of action observation and execution in 14-month-old infants: an event-related EEG desynchronization study. , 2011, Developmental science.

[34]  D. Schunk reflecting positive , 2022 .

[35]  Rongjun Yu,et al.  Brain responses to outcomes of one's own and other's performance in a gambling task , 2006, Neuroreport.

[36]  A. Sanfey,et al.  Independent Coding of Reward Magnitude and Valence in the Human Brain , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Kazuo Hiraki,et al.  Whose loss is it? Human electrophysiological correlates of non-self reward processing , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[38]  M. Nielsen,et al.  Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman Children and the Origins of Human Cultural Cognition , 2010, Psychological science.

[39]  A. V. van Duijvenvoorde,et al.  Evaluating the Negative or Valuing the Positive? Neural Mechanisms Supporting Feedback-Based Learning across Development , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Jutta Kray,et al.  Developmental differences in learning and error processing: evidence from ERPs. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[42]  S. Rombouts,et al.  Better than Expected or as Bad as You Thought? The Neurocognitive Development of Probabilistic Feedback Processing , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[43]  Christian Bellebaum,et al.  It was not MY fault: event-related brain potentials in active and observational learning from feedback. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  S. Hale,et al.  Relationships among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence in children , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[45]  Alexander M. Nitsch,et al.  A Single-Trial Estimation of the Feedback-Related Negativity and Its Relation to BOLD Responses in a Time-Estimation Task , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Guideline Thirteen: Guidelines for Standard Electrode Position Nomenclature , 1994, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[47]  S. Thompson Social Learning Theory , 2008 .