Effects of breaks and goal switches on the vigilance decrement
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] McCORMACK Pd,et al. Performance in a vigilance task as a function of interstimulus interval and interpolated rest. , 1958 .
[2] R. Parasuraman. Memory load and event rate control sensitivity decrements in sustained attention. , 1979, Science.
[3] Joel S. Warm,et al. Event-related cerebral hemodynamics reveal target-specific resource allocation for both “go” and “no-go” response-based vigilance tasks , 2013, Brain and Cognition.
[4] W. Helton,et al. Working memory load and the vigilance decrement , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[5] S. Malinen,et al. Post-disaster depression and vigilance: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] William S. Helton,et al. Visuospatial and verbal working memory load: effects on visuospatial vigilance , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[7] William S. Helton,et al. The effects of emotional stimuli on target detection: Indirect and direct resource costs , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.
[8] D. Washburn,et al. Task engagement, cerebral blood flow velocity, and diagnostic monitoring for sustained attention. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[9] C. Wickens,et al. Workload transition: Implications for individual and team performance. , 1993 .
[10] S. Hart,et al. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of Empirical and Theoretical Research , 1988 .
[11] G. Keppel. Data analysis for research designs , 1990 .
[12] Tyler H. Shaw,et al. Effects of sensory modality on cerebral blood flow velocity during vigilance , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.
[13] Judi E. See,et al. Meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. , 1995 .
[14] R. F. Thompson,et al. Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. , 1966, Psychological review.
[15] N. Mackworth. The Breakdown of Vigilance during Prolonged Visual Search 1 , 1948 .
[16] W. Helton,et al. Brief mental breaks and content-free cues may not keep you focused , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] P. Hancock,et al. Human Mental Workload , 1988 .
[18] Joel S. Warm,et al. Vigilance Requires Hard Mental Work and Is Stressful , 2008, Hum. Factors.
[19] G. Beauchamp,et al. Time for some a priori thinking about post hoc testing , 2008 .
[20] James L. Szalma,et al. The Vigilance Decrement Reflects Limitations in Effortful Attention, Not Mindlessness , 2003, Hum. Factors.
[21] John A. Detre,et al. Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental workload: An ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect , 2010, NeuroImage.
[22] Edward M. Hitchcock,et al. Automation cueing modulates cerebral blood flow and vigilance in a simulated air traffic control task , 2003 .
[23] Rebecca A. Grier,et al. Fundamental dimensions of subjective state in performance settings: task engagement, distress, and worry. , 2002, Emotion.
[24] B. Bergum,et al. Vigilance performance as a function of interpolated rest. , 1962 .
[25] Joel S. Warm,et al. Cerebral Hemodynamics and Vigilance Performance , 2009 .
[26] A. Chatterjee,et al. Energetic effects of stimulus intensity on prolonged simple reaction-time performance , 2010, Psychological research.
[27] P D McCORMACK. Performance in a vigilance task as a function of interstimulus interval and interpolated rest. , 1958, Canadian journal of psychology.
[28] A. G. Bills. Blocking: a new principle of mental fatigue. , 1931 .
[29] W. Helton,et al. Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of vigilance. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[30] Donald A. Wilson,et al. Habituation revisited: An updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[31] Kelley S. Parsons,et al. The abbreviated vigilance task and cerebral hemodynamics , 2007, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.
[32] S. Eickhoff,et al. Sustaining attention to simple tasks: a meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.
[33] J. F. Mackworth. Vigilance, arousal, and habituation. , 1968, Psychological review.
[34] W. Helton. The relationship between lateral differences in tympanic membrane temperature and behavioral impulsivity , 2010, Brain and Cognition.
[35] Joel S. Warm,et al. Effects of Cueing and Knowledge of Results on Workload and Boredom in Sustained Attention , 1999, Hum. Factors.
[36] Raul Ramirez,et al. Using cerebral hemovelocity to measure workload during a spatialised auditory vigilance task in novice and experienced observers , 2013, Ergonomics.
[37] Richard F. Thompson. Habituation: A history , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[38] W. Helton,et al. Target predictability, sustained attention, and response inhibition , 2013, Brain and Cognition.
[39] William S. Helton,et al. Sustained attention to local and global target features is different: Performance and tympanic membrane temperature , 2009, Brain and Cognition.
[40] A. Lleras,et al. Brief and rare mental “breaks” keep you focused: Deactivation and reactivation of task goals preempt vigilance decrements , 2010, Cognition.
[41] P. Hancock,et al. Cerebral lateralization of vigilance: A function of task difficulty , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[42] W. Helton,et al. Search asymmetry, sustained attention, and response inhibition , 2011, Brain and Cognition.
[43] Todd D. Hollander,et al. Signal regularity and the mindlessness model of vigilance. , 2005, British journal of psychology.
[44] Tyler H. Shaw,et al. Effects of regular or irregular event schedules on cerebral hemovelocity during a sustained attention task , 2012, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.
[45] William S. Helton,et al. A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study of sustained attention to local and global target features , 2013, Brain and Cognition.