Direction and distance deficits in path integration after unilateral vestibular loss depend on task complexity.

The effects of peripheral vestibular disorders on the direction and distance components of the internal spatial representation were investigated. The ability of Menière's patients to perform path integration was assessed in different situations aimed at differentiating the level of spatial processing (simple versus complex tasks), the available sensory cues (proprioceptive, vestibular, or visual conditions), and the side of the path (towards the healthy versus the lesioned side). After exploring two legs of a triangle, participants were required either to reproduce the exploration path, to follow the reverse path, or to take a shortcut to the starting point of the path (triangle completion). Patients' performances were recorded before unilateral vestibular neurotomy (UVN) and during the time-course of recovery (1 week and 1 month) and were compared to those of matched control subjects tested at similar time intervals. Both the angular and linear path components of the trajectory were impaired for patients compared to controls. However, deficits were restricted to the complex tasks, which required a higher level of spatial processing. Most deficits were maximal 1 week after UVN, and some remained up to the first post-operative month. Spatial representation was differentially impaired according to the available sensory cues: deficits were absent in active locomotor blindfolded condition, appeared in conditions involving visual and vestibular information, and were maximal when visual cues alone were available. Finally, concerning the side of the path, unilateral vestibular loss led to global impairment of the internal spatial representation, yet some asymmetrical spatial performances were observed 1 week after UVN. On the whole, results suggest that the environment experienced by the patients is different after UVN and that a different internal spatial representation is constructed, especially for tasks requiring high levels of spatial processing.

[1]  T. Brandt,et al.  Vestibulopathic gait. Walking and running. , 2001, Advances in neurology.

[2]  E. Batschelet Circular statistics in biology , 1981 .

[3]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Differential effects of labyrinthine dysfunction on distance and direction during blindfolded walking of a triangular path , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Locomotion Mode Affects the Updating of Objects Encountered During Travel: The Contribution of Vestibular and Proprioceptive Inputs to Path Integration , 1998, Presence.

[5]  F. Guedry Psychophysics of Vestibular Sensation , 1974 .

[6]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Homing by Path Integration , 1982 .

[7]  P Péruch,et al.  Homing in Virtual Environments: Effects of Field of View and Path Layout , 1997, Perception.

[8]  I. Curthoys,et al.  The effect of visual deprivation on vestibular compensation in the guinea pig , 1986, Brain Research.

[9]  W. E. Collins,et al.  Vestibular System Part 2: Psychophysics, Applied Aspects and General Interpretations , 2012, Handbook of Sensory Physiology.

[10]  Peter J. Werkhoven,et al.  The Effects of Proprioceptive and Visual Feedback on Geographical Orientation in Virtual Environments , 1999, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[11]  William H Warren,et al.  Path Integration from Optic Flow and Body Senses in a Homing Task , 2002, Perception.

[12]  V. V. Marlinsky,et al.  Vestibular and vestibulo-proprioceptive perception of motion in the horizontal plane in blindfolded man—III. Route inference , 1999, Neuroscience.

[13]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Encoding Spatial Representations Through Nonvisually Guided Locomotion: Tests of Human Path Integration , 1997 .

[14]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  P. Worchel,et al.  The role of the vestibular organs in space orientation. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  V. V. Marlinsky Vestibular and vestibulo-proprioceptive perception of motion in the horizontal plane in blindfolded man. Estimations of linear displacement , 1999, Neuroscience.

[18]  V. V. Marlinsky,et al.  Vestibular and vestibulo-proprioceptive perception of motion in the horizontal plane in blindfolded man—II. Estimations of rotations about the earth-vertical axis , 1999, Neuroscience.

[19]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Visual Homing Is Possible Without Landmarks: A Path Integration Study in Virtual Reality , 2002, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[20]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[21]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[22]  Y. Chan,et al.  Bilateral otolith contribution to spatial coding in the vestibular system. , 2002, Journal of biomedical science.

[23]  J. Rieser Access to Knowledge of Spatial Structure at Novel Points of Observation , 1989 .

[24]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Goal-directed linear locomotion in normal and labyrinthine-defective subjects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  P Péruch,et al.  Spatial performance of unilateral vestibular defective patients in nonvisual versus visual navigation. , 1999, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[26]  G M Jones,et al.  Vestibular-contingent voluntary saccades based on cognitive estimates of remembered vestibular information. , 1988, Advances in oto-rhino-laryngology.

[27]  M. Gresty,et al.  Self‐Controlled Reorienting Movements in Response to Rotational Displacements in Normal Subjects and Patients with Labyrinthine Disease , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[28]  T. Brandt,et al.  The Vestibular Cortex: Its Locations, Functions, and Disorders , 1999, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[29]  I S Curthoys,et al.  Vestibular compensation: a review of the oculomotor, neural, and clinical consequences of unilateral vestibular loss. , 1995, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[30]  W. Warren,et al.  Perception of translational heading from optical flow. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  I. Altman,et al.  Handbook of environmental psychology , 1987 .

[32]  M. Gresty,et al.  Evaluating 3D semicircular canal function by perception of rotation. , 1997, The American journal of otology.

[33]  A Berthoz,et al.  Gaze strategies during linear motion in head-free humans. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  A Berthoz,et al.  Spatial memory of body linear displacement: what is being stored? , 1995, Science.

[35]  M. Potegal Spatial abilities : development and physiological foundations , 1982 .

[36]  H S Cohen,et al.  Vestibular disorders and impaired path integration along a linear trajectory. , 2000, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[37]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Contribution of the otoliths to the calculation of linear displacement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  J. Thomson How do we use visual information to control locomotion? , 1980, Trends in Neurosciences.

[39]  Christophe Lopez,et al.  Walking performance of vestibular-defective patients before and after unilateral vestibular neurotomy , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.