Effects of contextual relevance on pragmatic inference during conversation: An fMRI study

&NA; Contextual relevance, which is vital for understanding conversational implicatures (CI), engages both the frontal‐temporal language and theory‐of‐mind networks. Here we investigate how contextual relevance affects CI processing and regulates the connectivity between CI‐processing‐related brain regions. Participants listened to dialogues in which the level of contextual relevance to dialogue‐final utterance (reply) was manipulated. This utterance was either direct, indirect but relevant, irrelevant with contextual hint, or irrelevant with no contextual hint. Results indicated that compared with direct replies, indirect replies showed increased activations in bilateral IFG, bilateral MTG, bilateral TPJ, dmPFC, and precuneus, and increased connectivity between rTPJ/dmPFC and both IFG and MTG. Moreover, irrelevant replies activated right MTG along an anterior‐posterior gradient as a function of the level of irrelevance. Our study provides novel evidence concerning how the language and theory‐of‐mind networks interact for pragmatic inference and how the processing of CI is modulated by level of contextual relevance. HighlightsDecreasing contextual relevance activates right MTG along an anterior‐posterior gradient.Interaction between language and mentalizing networks subserves pragmatic inference.

[1]  Michael Erb,et al.  Laterality in metaphor processing: Lack of evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging for the right hemisphere theory , 2007, Brain and Language.

[2]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[3]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Psychophysiological and Modulatory Interactions in Neuroimaging , 1997, NeuroImage.

[4]  Mante S. Nieuwland,et al.  On sense and reference: Examining the functional neuroanatomy of referential processing , 2007, NeuroImage.

[5]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  R. Saxe,et al.  Theory of Mind: A Neural Prediction Problem , 2013, Neuron.

[7]  Hae-Jeong Park,et al.  Everyday conversation requires cognitive inference: Neural bases of comprehending implicated meanings in conversations , 2013, NeuroImage.

[8]  M. Erb,et al.  Neural correlates of metaphor processing. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[9]  D. Sperber,et al.  Relevance: Communication and Cognition , 1989 .

[10]  Peter Hagoort,et al.  When Elephants Fly: Differential Sensitivity of Right and Left Inferior Frontal Gyri to Discourse and World Knowledge , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  Peter Hagoort,et al.  MUC (Memory, Unification, Control) and beyond , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[12]  Jun-ichi Abe,et al.  Neural processing associated with comprehension of an indirect reply during a scenario reading task , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Robyn Carston,et al.  Relevance Theory and the saying/implicating distinction , 2008 .

[14]  Irene P. Kan,et al.  Selection from perceptual and conceptual representations , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  Peter Hagoort,et al.  Beyond the language given: the neural correlates of inferring speaker meaning. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[17]  Tokiko Harada,et al.  Distinction between the literal and intended meanings of sentences: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of metaphor and sarcasm , 2012, Cortex.

[18]  Phillip J. Holcomb,et al.  Making sense of discourse: An fMRI study of causal inferencing across sentences , 2006, NeuroImage.

[19]  Ingrid R. Olson,et al.  Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes , 2010, NeuroImage.

[20]  S. Huettel,et al.  A Distinct Role of the Temporal-Parietal Junction in Predicting Socially Guided Decisions , 2012, Science.

[21]  Kent Bach,et al.  The top 10 misconceptions about implicature , 2006 .

[22]  T. Landauer,et al.  Indexing by Latent Semantic Analysis , 1990 .

[23]  Daniel S. Margulies,et al.  Prioritizing spatial accuracy in high-resolution fMRI data using multivariate feature weight mapping , 2014, Front. Neurosci..

[24]  P. Roland,et al.  Estimation of the Probabilities of 3D Clusters in Functional Brain Images , 1998, NeuroImage.

[25]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Recovering Meaning Left Prefrontal Cortex Guides Controlled Semantic Retrieval , 2001, Neuron.

[26]  G. Bush,et al.  The Multi-Source Interference Task: an fMRI task that reliably activates the cingulo-frontal-parietal cognitive/attention network , 2006, Nature Protocols.

[27]  L. Aravind,et al.  Integration of Word Meaning and World Knowledge in Language Comprehension , 2022 .

[28]  Peter Hagoort,et al.  A job interview in the MRI scanner: How does indirectness affect addressees and overhearers? , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Jiang Xu,et al.  Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension , 2005, NeuroImage.

[31]  D. Shankweiler,et al.  An Event-related Neuroimaging Study Distinguishing Form and Content in Sentence Processing , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Jérôme Prado,et al.  Neural evidence that utterance-processing entails mentalizing: The case of irony , 2012, NeuroImage.

[33]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Unified segmentation , 2005, NeuroImage.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Multisubject fMRI Studies and Conjunction Analyses , 1999, NeuroImage.

[35]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  The extended language network: A meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[36]  Diana Van Lancker Sidtis,et al.  Where in the brain is nonliteral language , 2006 .

[37]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Differential Contributions of Bilateral Ventral Anterior Temporal Lobe and Left Anterior Superior Temporal Gyrus to Semantic Processes , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  Florian Th. Siebörger,et al.  Making sense of nonsense: An fMRI study of task induced inference processes during discourse comprehension , 2007, Brain Research.

[39]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[40]  Michael Erb,et al.  Where in the brain is nonliteral language? A coordinate-based meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies , 2012, NeuroImage.

[41]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Oury Monchi,et al.  Function of basal ganglia in bridging cognitive and motor modules to perform an action , 2014, Front. Neurosci..

[43]  Mante S. Nieuwland,et al.  Establishing propositional truth-value in counterfactual and real-world contexts during sentence comprehension: Differential sensitivity of the left and right inferior frontal gyri , 2012, NeuroImage.

[44]  Donald W. Pfaff,et al.  Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  M. Hentze,et al.  Making sense of nonsense , 2013, Nature Structural &Molecular Biology.

[46]  Hsuan-Chih Chen,et al.  The anterior left inferior frontal gyrus contributes to semantic unification , 2012, NeuroImage.

[47]  Benjamin Martin Bly,et al.  The Brain Basis of Syntactic Processes , 1999 .

[48]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Neural Dynamics of Speech Act Comprehension: An MEG Study of Naming and Requesting , 2013, Brain Topography.

[49]  M. Beeman,et al.  Complementary Right- and Left-Hemisphere Language Comprehension , 1998 .

[50]  A. Friederici,et al.  The brain basis of syntactic processes: functional imaging and lesion studies , 2003, NeuroImage.

[51]  David N. Lee,et al.  Function of basal ganglia in tauG-guiding action , 2017, bioRxiv.

[52]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  The role of coherence and cohesion in text comprehension: an event-related fMRI study. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[53]  S. Huettel,et al.  A nexus model of the temporal–parietal junction , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  Shirley-Ann Rueschemeyer,et al.  Neuronal interactions between mentalising and action systems during indirect request processing. , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[55]  R. Mar The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[56]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[57]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Looking at the brains behind figurative language—A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on metaphor, idiom, and irony processing , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[58]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[59]  S. Bookheimer Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[60]  M. Farah,et al.  Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[61]  H. Grice Logic and conversation , 1975 .

[62]  Daniel Casasanto,et al.  Pragmatics in Action: Indirect Requests Engage Theory of Mind Areas and the Cortical Motor Network , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  M. Jung-Beeman Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.