Evidence for enhanced discrimination of virtual auditory distance among blind listeners using level and direct-to-reverberant cues

Totally blind listeners often demonstrate better than normal capabilities when performing spatial hearing tasks. Accurate representation of three-dimensional auditory space requires the processing of available distance information between the listener and the sound source; however, auditory distance cues vary greatly depending upon the acoustic properties of the environment, and it is not known which distance cues are important to totally blind listeners. Our data show that totally blind listeners display better performance compared to sighted age-matched controls for distance discrimination tasks in anechoic and reverberant virtual rooms simulated using a room-image procedure. Totally blind listeners use two major auditory distance cues to stationary sound sources, level and direct-to-reverberant ratio, more effectively than sighted controls for many of the virtual distances tested. These results show that significant compensation among totally blind listeners for virtual auditory spatial distance leads to benefits across a range of simulated acoustic environments. No significant differences in performance were observed between listeners with partial non-correctable visual losses and sighted controls, suggesting that sensory compensation for virtual distance does not occur for listeners with partial vision loss.

[1]  Anne Marsden,et al.  International Organization for Standardization , 2014 .

[2]  M. Lassonde,et al.  Blind subjects process auditory spectral cues more efficiently than sighted individuals , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  F L Wightman,et al.  Localization using nonindividualized head-related transfer functions. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  P. W. Smith,et al.  Comments on “New Method of Measuring Reverberation Time” [M. R. Schroeder, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 37, 409–412 (1965)] , 1965 .

[5]  Daphne Bavelier,et al.  I see where you're hearing: how cross-modal plasticity may exploit homologous brain structures , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  K. Yuen,et al.  Correction: Design of Wide-Spectrum Inhibitors Targeting Coronavirus Main Proteases , 2005, PLoS Biology.

[7]  Mats E Nilsson,et al.  Human Echolocation: Blind and Sighted Persons' Ability to Detect Sounds Recorded in the Presence of a Reflecting Object , 2010, Perception.

[8]  Michael A Akeroyd,et al.  The detection of differences in the cues to distance by elderly hearing-impaired listeners. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  W. G. Gardner,et al.  HRTF measurements of a KEMAR , 1995 .

[10]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Relevance of Spectral Cues for Auditory Spatial Processing in the Occipital Cortex of the Blind , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[11]  Santani Teng,et al.  The acuity of echolocation: Spatial resolution in the sighted compared to expert performance. , 2011, Journal of visual impairment & blindness.

[12]  Pavel Zahorik,et al.  Perceptually relevant parameters for virtual listening simulation of small room acoustics. , 2009, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  M. Paré,et al.  Early-blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects , 1998, Nature.

[14]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Early- and Late-Onset Blind Individuals Show Supra-Normal Auditory Abilities in Far-Space , 2004, Current Biology.

[15]  Pavel Zahorik,et al.  Direct-to-reverberant energy ratio sensitivity. , 2002, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  R. Zatorre,et al.  A Functional Neuroimaging Study of Sound Localization: Visual Cortex Activity Predicts Performance in Early-Blind Individuals , 2005, PLoS biology.

[17]  E. Lehmann,et al.  Prediction of energy decay in room impulse responses simulated with an image-source model. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  P. Coleman An analysis of cues to auditory depth perception in free space. , 1963, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  Makoto Otani,et al.  Numerical study on source-distance dependency of head-related transfer functions. , 2009, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  Adria E. N. Hoover,et al.  Sensory compensation in sound localization in people with one eye , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Pavel Zahorik,et al.  Assessing auditory distance perception using virtual acoustics. , 2002, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  Mats E Nilsson,et al.  Human Echolocation: Pitch versus Loudness Information , 2011, Perception.

[23]  Barbara G Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Effect of stimulus spectrum on distance perception for nearby sources. , 2011, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  J. Lewald Gender-specific hemispheric asymmetry in auditory space perception. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[25]  D. Brungart,et al.  The effects of production and presentation level on the auditory distance perception of speech. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  Adelstein Am,et al.  Revision of the international classification of diseases. , 1971, Lancet.

[27]  Tammo Houtgast,et al.  Auditory distance perception in rooms , 1999, Nature.

[28]  P. Divenyi,et al.  Lateralization of Narrow-Band Noise by Blind and Sighted Listeners , 2002, Perception.

[29]  Victor Candas,et al.  Enhanced sensitivity to echo cues in blind subjects , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  C Veraart,et al.  Processing of auditory information by the blind in spatial localization tasks , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  A. D. Little,et al.  Effects of Room Reflectance and Background Noise on Perceived Auditory Distance , 1989, Perception.

[32]  R J Ilmoniemi,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for cross-modal plasticity in humans with early- and late-onset blindness. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[33]  David Guth,et al.  Echolocation Reconsidered: Using Spatial Variations in the Ambient Sound Field to Guide Locomotion , 1998 .

[34]  Nandini Iyer,et al.  On the minimum audible difference in direct-to-reverberant energy ratio. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[35]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Adaptation to sensory loss. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[36]  M. Schroeder New Method of Measuring Reverberation Time , 1965 .

[37]  David Whitney,et al.  Ultrafine spatial acuity of blind expert human echolocators , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  A. Dufour,et al.  Improved auditory spatial sensitivity in near-sighted subjects. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[39]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Organization and Reorganization of Sensory-Deprived Cortex , 2012, Current Biology.

[40]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans , 1999, Nature.

[41]  Werner Lutzenberger,et al.  The right supratemporal plane hears the distance of objects: neuromagnetic correlates of virtual reality , 2003, Neuroreport.

[42]  M. Goodale,et al.  Citation for Published Item: Use Policy Neural Correlates of Natural Human Echolocation in Early and Late Blind Echolocation Experts , 2022 .

[43]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[44]  C Muchnik,et al.  Central auditory skills in blind and sighted subjects. , 1991, Scandinavian audiology.

[45]  D H Mershon,et al.  Absolute and Relative Cues for the Auditory Perception of Egocentric Distance , 1979, Perception.

[46]  S. Lomber,et al.  Cross-modal plasticity in specific auditory cortices underlies visual compensations in the deaf , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  A. Bronkhorst,et al.  Auditory distance perception in humans : A summary of past and present research , 2005 .

[48]  M. Lassonde,et al.  Cross-modal plasticity for the spatial processing of sounds in visually deprived subjects , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Differential occipital responses in early- and late-blind individuals during a sound-source discrimination task , 2008, NeuroImage.

[50]  A. Stevens,et al.  Auditory perceptual consolidation in early-onset blindness , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  Manabu Honda,et al.  Critical Period for Cross-Modal Plasticity in Blind Humans: A Functional MRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[52]  W M Hartmann,et al.  On the externalization of sound images. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[53]  N. Prins Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction , 2009 .

[54]  A. Dufour,et al.  Auditory compensation in myopic humans: involvement of binaural, monaural, or echo cues? , 2005, Brain Research.