Modulation of Brain Activity during a Stroop Inhibitory Task by the Kind of Cognitive Control Required
暂无分享,去创建一个
Christophe Phillips | Evelyne Balteau | André Luxen | Christian Degueldre | Pierre Maquet | Eric Salmon | Fabienne Collette | C. Degueldre | E. Balteau | C. Phillips | A. Luxen | P. Maquet | F. Collette | K. D'ostilio | E. Salmon | Julien Grandjean | Kevin D’Ostilio | J. Grandjean
[1] V. Menon,et al. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.
[2] Chris Blais,et al. Behavioral and Neural Evidence for Item-specific Performance Monitoring , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[3] Gordon D. Logan,et al. Stroop-Type Interference : Congruity Effects in Color Naming With Typewritten Responses , 1998 .
[4] Eliot Hazeltine,et al. Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.
[5] J. Ridley. Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .
[6] Antonino Vallesi,et al. Task Context and Frontal Lobe Activation in the Stroop Task , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[7] D. Lowe,et al. Selective and divided Attention in a Stroop task. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.
[8] L. Jacoby,et al. Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task , 2008, Memory & cognition.
[9] T. L. Brown. The relationship between Stroop interference and facilitation effects: statistical artifacts, baselines, and a reassessment. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] R Turner,et al. Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T , 2004, NeuroImage.
[11] Andrew R. A. Conway,et al. Variation in working memory , 2008 .
[12] T. Braver,et al. Motivated Cognitive Control: Reward Incentives Modulate Preparatory Neural Activity during Task-Switching , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[13] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.
[14] Michael H. Buonocore,et al. Integrating Conflict Detection and Attentional Control Mechanisms , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[15] S. Gauthier,et al. Inhibition impairments in Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment and healthy aging: Effect of congruency proportion in a Stroop task , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[16] Koji Jimura,et al. Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task switching. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.
[17] Hannah S. Locke,et al. Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[18] T. Braver,et al. Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. , 2007 .
[19] Tobias Egner,et al. The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task , 2005, NeuroImage.
[20] A. MacDonald,et al. The neural basis of cognitive control: Response selection and inhibition , 2009, Brain and Cognition.
[21] Kathryn M. McMillan,et al. A comparison of label‐based review and ALE meta‐analysis in the Stroop task , 2005, Human brain mapping.
[22] J. Cohen,et al. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.
[23] G. Logan,et al. When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .
[24] Colin M. Macleod,et al. Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[25] Todd S. Braver,et al. A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions , 2006, Neurocomputing.
[26] B. Forstmann,et al. Neurocognitive mechanisms of action control: resisting the call of the Sirens. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.
[27] K. Hutchison,et al. The Interactive Effects of Listwide Control, Item-based Control, and Working Memory Capacity on Stroop Performance and Holly Vander Vos for Scheduling and Running Participants for This Study. I Am Also Grateful To , 2022 .
[28] K. Zilles,et al. A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.
[29] T. Braver,et al. Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and prefrontal function in healthy aging. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[30] Colin M. Macleod,et al. Training and Stroop-like interference: evidence for a continuum of automaticity. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[31] L. Jacoby,et al. Stroop process dissociations: the relationship between facilitation and interference. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] Tor D. Wager,et al. Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI , 2005, NeuroImage.
[33] T. Braver. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[34] Jin Fan,et al. Cognitive and Brain Consequences of Conflict , 2003, NeuroImage.
[35] Hannah S. Locke,et al. Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human prefrontal cortex , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[36] Hannah S. Locke,et al. Motivational influences on cognitive control: Behavior, brain activation, and individual differences , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[37] Derek Besner,et al. Item-specific adaptation and the conflict-monitoring hypothesis: a computational model. , 2007, Psychological review.
[38] James L. McClelland,et al. On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.
[39] M. Botvinick,et al. Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[40] J. Jonides,et al. Interference resolution: Insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[41] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[42] Deanna M. Barch,et al. Accounting for Cognitive Aging: Context Processing, Inhibition or Processing Speed? , 2006, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.
[43] R. Engle,et al. Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[44] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[45] Jesper Andersson,et al. Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.
[46] Gordon D. Logan,et al. Stroop-Type Interference: Congruity Effects in Color Naming With Typewritten Responses , 1998 .
[47] Ardi Roelofs,et al. Attentional Control in Anterior Cingulate Cortex Based on Probabilistic Cueing , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[48] Katherine L. Roberts,et al. Examining a Supramodal Network for Conflict Processing: A Systematic Review and Novel Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data for Related Visual and Auditory Stroop Tasks , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[49] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[50] J. Bugg,et al. List-wide control is not entirely elusive: Evidence from picture–word Stroop , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[51] M. McDaniel,et al. Revealing list-level control in the Stroop task by uncovering its benefits and a cost. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.