“Conflicting” Motion Cues to the Visual and Vestibular Self- Motion Systems Around 0.06 Hz Evoke Simulator Sickness

The basic question this research addressed was, how does simulator sickness vary with simulated motion frequency? Participants were 11 women and 19 men, 20 to 63 years of age. A visual self-motion frequency response curve was determined using a Chattecx posture platform with a VR4 head-mounted display (HMD) or a back-projected dome. That curve and one for vestibular self-motion specify a frequency range in which vestibular and visual motion stimuli could produce conflicting self-motion cues. Using a rotating chair and the HMD, a third experiment supported (p < .01) the hypothesis that conflicting cues at the frequency of maximum “crossover” between the curves (about 0.06 Hz) would be more likely to evoke simulator sickness than would conflicting cues at a higher frequency. Actual or potential applications of this work include a preliminary design guidance curve that indicates the frequency range of simulated motion that is likely to evoke simulator or virtual reality sickness; for simulators intended to operate in this frequency range, appropriate simulator sickness interventions should be considered during the design process.

[1]  T. Stoffregen,et al.  Postural instability precedes motion sickness , 1998, Brain Research Bulletin.

[2]  James R. Lackner,et al.  Circumventing Side Effects of Immersive Virtual Environments , 1997, HCI.

[3]  J T Reason,et al.  Motion Sickness Adaptation: A Neural Mismatch Model 1 , 1978, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[4]  Robert S. Kennedy,et al.  Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. , 1993 .

[5]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  The role of vision in the control of posture during linear motion. , 1979, Progress in brain research.

[6]  J. Moran,et al.  Sensation and perception , 1980 .

[7]  Kay M. Stanney,et al.  Postural instability induced by virtual reality exposure: Development of a certification protocol , 1996, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[8]  K. Hamilton,et al.  Limitations of postural equilibrium tests for examining simulator sickness. , 1989, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[9]  W. Bles,et al.  Motion sickness. , 2000, Current opinion in neurology.

[10]  V. Henn,et al.  Neuronal activity in the vestibular nuclei of the alert monkey during vestibular and optokinetic stimulation , 1977, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Michael W. Haas,et al.  Postural Instability and Motion Sickness in a Fixed-Base Flight Simulator , 2000, Hum. Factors.

[12]  S. C. Wong,et al.  Subjective motion and acceleration induced by the movement of the observer’s entire visual field , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[14]  F. R. C. S. Edin The Vestibular Apparatus , 1920, Edinburgh Medical Journal.

[15]  M J Griffin,et al.  Effect of seating, vision and direction of horizontal oscillation on motion sickness. , 2000, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[16]  R N Haber,et al.  Flight simulation. , 1986, Scientific American.

[17]  M. Sanders Handbook of Sensory Physiology , 1975 .

[18]  James Park,et al.  The Brain's Sense of Movement , 2003, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[19]  T A Furness,et al.  The use of an independent visual background to reduce simulator side-effects. , 1999, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[20]  J F Golding,et al.  A motion sickness maximum around the 0.2 Hz frequency range of horizontal translational oscillation. , 2001, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[21]  J H Mackintosh,et al.  The effect of altitude on tests of reaction time and alertness. , 1988, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[22]  R S Kennedy,et al.  Disorientation and postural ataxia following flight simulation. , 1997, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[23]  Thomas A. Furness,et al.  A “Natural” Independent Visual Background Reduced Simulator Sickness , 2002 .

[24]  J H Milsum,et al.  Spatial and dynamic aspects of visual fixation. , 1965, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[25]  T. Stoffregen,et al.  An ecological Theory of Motion Sickness and Postural Instability , 1991 .

[26]  F. Guedry Psychophysics of Vestibular Sensation , 1974 .

[27]  P. E. K. D.,et al.  Biological control systems analysis , 2006, Medical and biological engineering.

[28]  Gavriel Salvendy,et al.  Aftereffects and Sense of Presence in Virtual Environments: Formulation of a Research and Development Agenda , 1998, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[29]  M J Griffin,et al.  Prediction of the incidence of motion sickness from the magnitude, frequency, and duration of vertical oscillation. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[30]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Visual-Vestibular Interaction: Effects on Self-Motion Perception and Postural Control , 1978 .

[31]  R. So,et al.  Cybersickness in the presence of scene rotational movements along different axes. , 2001, Applied ergonomics.

[32]  D E Parker,et al.  Human Vestibular Function and Weightlessness , 1991, Journal of clinical pharmacology.

[33]  Henry Been-Lirn Duh,et al.  Use of an independent visual background to alleviate simulator sickness in the virtual environments that employ wide-field displays , 2001 .

[34]  H. Bastian Sensation and Perception.—I , 1869, Nature.