Receiver psychology and the design of the deceptive caudal luring signal of the death adder

Signal design can reflect the sensory properties of receivers. The death adder, Acanthophis antarcticus, attracts prey by wriggling the distal portion of its tail (caudal luring). To understand the design of this deceptive signal, we explored perceptual processes in a representative prey species: the Jacky dragon, Amphibolurus muricatus. We used 3D animations of fast and slow death adder luring movements against different backgrounds, to test the hypothesis that caudal luring mimics salient aspects of invertebrate prey. Moving stimuli elicited predatory responses, especially against a conspicuous background. To identify putative models for caudal luring, we used an optic flow algorithm to extract velocity values from video sequences of 61 moving invertebrates caught in lizard territories, and compared these to the velocity values of death adder movements. Caudal lures had motion characteristics that matched common invertebrate prey speeds, each corresponding to a peak in a bimodal distribution. Subsequent video playback tests using animations of crickets showed that significantly more attacks were evoked by stimuli moving at common than at rare invertebrate speeds. Overall, these results suggest that biases in the nervous system of the receiver, originally selected for prey recognition, might have been exploited by the design of the caudal luring signal. We suggest that viewing caudal luring from this perspective, rather than thinking of it as aggressive mimicry per se, may help us understand the function and evolutionary origin of this behaviour.

[1]  J. Krebs,et al.  Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach , 1978 .

[2]  P. McGregor,et al.  Considerations on the use of video playbacks as visual stimuli: the Lisbon workshop consensus , 2000, acta ethologica.

[3]  Richard A Peters,et al.  Active space of a movement-based signal: response to the Jacky dragon (Amphibolurus muricatus) display is sensitive to distance, but independent of orientation , 2007, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[4]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[5]  Paul A. Carlile,et al.  Detection of a looming stimulus by the Jacky dragon: selective sensitivity to characteristics of an aerial predator , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  G. W. Schuett,et al.  Aggressive mimicry in neonates of the sidewinder rattlesnake, Crotalus cerastes (Serpentes: Viperidae): stimulus control and visual perception of prey luring , 2008 .

[7]  C. Evans,et al.  Assessment of repeated displays: a test of possible mechanisms , 2007, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[8]  Darren Burke,et al.  Motion sensitivity of the Jacky dragon, Amphibolurus muricatus: random-dot kinematograms reveal the importance of motion noise for signal detection , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  S. Bradshaw,et al.  Feeding behavior and the occurrence of caudal luring in Burton's pygopodid Lialis burtonis (Sauria : Pygopodidae) , 1991 .

[10]  R. Dawkins,et al.  Animal signals: information or manipulation? , 1978 .

[11]  Richard A Peters,et al.  Introductory tail-flick of the Jacky dragon visual display: signal efficacy depends upon duration , 2003, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[12]  C. Clifford,et al.  Measuring the structure of dynamic visual signals , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  R. Peters,et al.  The Effect of Variation in Prey Movement on the Predatory Response of Jacky Lizards (Amphibolurus muricatus) , 2008 .

[14]  O Hasson,et al.  Pursuit-deterrent signals: communication between prey and predator. , 1991, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[15]  F. Kraus,et al.  Feeding mimicry in the rattlesnake Sistrurus catenatus, with comments on the evolution of the rattle , 1984, Animal Behaviour.

[16]  R. A. Peters,et al.  Design of the Jacky dragon visual display: signal and noise characteristics in a complex moving environment , 2003, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[17]  T. Guilford,et al.  Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  C. W. Radcliffe,et al.  Caudal luring in the southern death adder acanthophis antarcticus , 1990 .

[19]  A. Zahavi Mate selection-a selection for a handicap. , 1975, Journal of theoretical biology.

[20]  Life history and social learning: megapode chicks fail to acquire feeding preferences from conspecifics. , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.

[21]  R. Peters,et al.  Display response of the Jacky Dragon, Amphibolurus muricatus (Lacertilia: Agamidae), to intruders: A semi-Markovian process , 2003 .

[22]  Darren Burke,et al.  Technique for measuring speed and visual motion sensitivity in lizards , 2008 .

[23]  C. Carpenter,et al.  Tail Luring in the Death Adder, Acanthophis antarcticus (Reptilia, Serpentes, Elapidae) , 1978 .

[24]  David W. Hosmer,et al.  Applied Survival Analysis: Regression Modeling of Time-to-Event Data , 2008 .

[25]  J. Endler Signals, Signal Conditions, and the Direction of Evolution , 1992, The American Naturalist.

[26]  D. P. Wilson The Habits of the Angler-fish, Lophius piscatorius L., in the Plymouth Aquarium , 1937, Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom.

[27]  R L Williams,et al.  A Note on Robust Variance Estimation for Cluster‐Correlated Data , 2000, Biometrics.

[28]  H. Tiebout Caudal Luring by a Temperate Colubrid Snake, Elaphe obsoleta, and Its Implications for the Evolution of the Rattle among Rattlesnakes , 1997 .

[29]  H. Cogger Reptiles and Amphibians of Australia , 1975 .

[30]  M. Leal,et al.  Notes on the Feeding Behavior and Caudal Luring by Juvenile Alsophis portoricensis (Serpentes: Colubridae) , 1994 .

[31]  Daniel A. Van Dyk,et al.  Familiar–unfamiliar discrimination based on visual cues in the Jacky dragon, Amphibolurus muricatus , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[32]  T. Ord,et al.  Display rate and opponent assessment in the Jacky dragon (Amphibolurus muricatus): An experimental analysis , 2003 .

[33]  R. Peters,et al.  Digital video playback and visual communication in lizards , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[34]  R. Shine,et al.  Tails of enticement: caudal luring by an ambush-foraging snake (Acanthophis praelongus, Elapidae) , 2008 .

[35]  Andrew C. Mason,et al.  Measuring and quantifying dynamic visual signals in jumping spiders , 2006 .

[36]  Christopher S. Evans,et al.  Animal Acoustic Communication: Sound Analysis and Research Methods , 2011 .

[37]  I. Sazima Caudal luring in two neotropical pitvipers, Bothrops jararaca and B. jararacussu , 1991 .