Exaggeration of Language-Specific Rhythms in English and French Children's Songs

The available evidence indicates that the music of a culture reflects the speech rhythm of the prevailing language. The normalized pairwise variability index (nPVI) is a measure of durational contrast between successive events that can be applied to vowels in speech and to notes in music. Music–language parallels may have implications for the acquisition of language and music, but it is unclear whether native-language rhythms are reflected in children's songs. In general, children's songs exhibit greater rhythmic regularity than adults' songs, in line with their caregiving goals and frequent coordination with rhythmic movement. Accordingly, one might expect lower nPVI values (i.e., lower variability) for such songs regardless of culture. In addition to their caregiving goals, children's songs may serve an intuitive didactic function by modeling culturally relevant content and structure for music and language. One might therefore expect pronounced rhythmic parallels between children's songs and language of origin. To evaluate these predictions, we analyzed a corpus of 269 English and French songs from folk and children's music anthologies. As in prior work, nPVI values were significantly higher for English than for French children's songs. For folk songs (i.e., songs not for children), the difference in nPVI for English and French songs was small and in the expected direction but non-significant. We subsequently collected ratings from American and French monolingual and bilingual adults, who rated their familiarity with each song, how much they liked it, and whether or not they thought it was a children's song. Listeners gave higher familiarity and liking ratings to songs from their own culture, and they gave higher familiarity and preference ratings to children's songs than to other songs. Although higher child-directedness ratings were given to children's than to folk songs, French listeners drove this effect, and their ratings were uniquely predicted by nPVI. Together, these findings suggest that language-based rhythmic structures are evident in children's songs, and that listeners expect exaggerated language-based rhythms in children's songs. The implications of these findings for enculturation processes and for the acquisition of music and language are discussed.

[1]  E K Vassar,et al.  The Beginnings , 2020, Unstoppable.

[2]  I. Király,et al.  Preschoolers have better long-term memory for rhyming text than adults. , 2017, Developmental science.

[3]  I. Peretz,et al.  Singing Delays the Onset of Infant Distress , 2016 .

[4]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Musical intervention enhances infants’ neural processing of temporal structure in music and speech , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  E. Spelke,et al.  For 5-Month-Old Infants, Melodies Are Social , 2016, Psychological science.

[6]  R. de Diego-Balaguer,et al.  Temporal Attention as a Scaffold for Language Development , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[7]  Christina M. Vanden Bosch der Nederlanden,et al.  Finding the music of speech: Musical knowledge influences pitch processing in speech , 2015, Cognition.

[8]  Thomas E. Currie,et al.  Statistical universals reveal the structures and functions of human music , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  S. Trehub,et al.  Musical affect regulation in infancy , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[10]  Alejandrina Cristia,et al.  Acoustic-phonetic differences between infant- and adult-directed speech: the role of stress and utterance position , 2014, Journal of Child Language.

[11]  Joel S. Snyder,et al.  Tapping to a Slow Tempo in the Presence of Simple and Complex Meters Reveals Experience-Specific Biases for Processing Music , 2014, PloS one.

[12]  A. Tremblay Native listening: Language experience and the recognition of spoken words by Anne Cutler (review) , 2014 .

[13]  J. Werker,et al.  Culture as a binder for bilingual acquisition , 2014, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[14]  E. Hannon,et al.  Linking prenatal experience to the emerging musical mind , 2013, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[15]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  An Empirical Study of Historical Patterns in Musical Rhythm: Analysis of German & Italian Classical Music Using the nPVI Equation , 2013 .

[16]  R. Mazuka,et al.  Is the mora rhythm of Japanese more strongly observed in infant-directed speech than in adult-directed speech? , 2013 .

[17]  Marijke A. K. A. Braeken,et al.  Detecting violations of temporal regularities in waking and sleeping two-month-old infants , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[18]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Native Listening: Language Experience and the Recognition of Spoken Words , 2012 .

[19]  E. Hannon,et al.  Effects of perceptual experience on children's and adults’ perception of unfamiliar rhythms , 2012, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[20]  E. Hannon,et al.  Familiarity overrides complexity in rhythm perception: a cross-cultural comparison of American and Turkish listeners. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  D. Temperley,et al.  Music-Language Correlations and the “Scotch Snap” , 2011 .

[22]  Katherine Jones,et al.  Rhythmic Refinements to the nPVI Measure: A Reanalysis of Patel & Daniele (2003a) , 2011 .

[23]  E. Hannon,et al.  Constraints on infants' musical rhythm perception: effects of interval ratio complexity and enculturation. , 2011, Developmental science.

[24]  Inês Salselas,et al.  Music and speech in early development: automatic analysis and classification of prosodic features from two Portuguese variants , 2011 .

[25]  A. Levitt,et al.  A Comparison of Rhythm in English Dialects and Music , 2011 .

[26]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Sing that tune: infants' perception of melody and lyrics and the facilitation of phonetic recognition in songs. , 2010, Infant behavior & development.

[27]  L. Trainor,et al.  Effects of Kindermusik training on infants' rhythmic enculturation. , 2010, Developmental science.

[28]  Leigh VanHandel,et al.  The Role of Meter in Compositional Style in 19th Century French and German Art Song , 2010 .

[29]  J. Fletcher The Prosody of Speech: Timing and Rhythm , 2010 .

[30]  E. Hannon,et al.  Perceiving speech rhythm in music: Listeners classify instrumental songs according to language of origin , 2009, Cognition.

[31]  E. Longhi `Songese': maternal structuring of musical interaction with infants , 2009 .

[32]  S. Kita,et al.  Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning , 2008, Cognition.

[33]  Martin Clayton,et al.  Time in Indian Music: Rhythm, Metre, and Form in North Indian Rag Performance , 2008 .

[34]  Laurence White,et al.  Calibrating rhythm: First language and second language studies , 2007, J. Phonetics.

[35]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  Comparing the rhythm and melody of speech and music: the case of British English and French. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[36]  S. Trehub,et al.  Tuning in to musical rhythms: infants learn more readily than adults. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and melodies: Implications for musical structure learning , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  P. J. Brooks,et al.  Diminutives in child-directed speech supplement metric with distributional word segmentation cues , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[39]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  Infant-Directed Speech Facilitates Word Segmentation. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[40]  S. Trehub,et al.  Metrical Categories in Infancy and Adulthood , 2005, Psychological science.

[41]  Shimon Ullman,et al.  Learning to Segment , 2004, ECCV.

[42]  Peter Ford Dominey,et al.  Indeterminacy in language acquisition: the role of child directed speech and joint attention , 2004, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[43]  K. Szpunar,et al.  Liking and memory for musical stimuli as a function of exposure. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[44]  David Huron,et al.  Agogic Contrast in French and English Themes: Further Support for Patel and Daniele (2003) , 2003 .

[45]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  When cues collide: use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[46]  Aniruddh D. Patel,et al.  An empirical comparison of rhythm in language and music , 2003, Cognition.

[47]  P. Bohlman Time in Indian Music: Rhythm, Metre, and Form in North Indian Rāg Performance . By Martin Clayton. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2000. xx, 230 pp., CD. $74.00 (cloth). , 2003 .

[48]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Word Segmentation by 8-Month-Olds: When Speech Cues Count More Than Statistics , 2001 .

[49]  Lisa D. Sanders,et al.  Lexical, syntactic, and stress-pattern cues for speech segmentation. , 2000, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[50]  Low Ee Ling,et al.  Q uantitative Characterizations of Speech Rhythm: Syllable-Timing in Singapore English , 2000, Language and speech.

[51]  David Temperley,et al.  Meter and Grouping in African Music: A View from Music Theory , 2000 .

[52]  F. Ramus,et al.  Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal , 1999, Cognition.

[53]  Mary R. Newsome,et al.  The Beginnings of Word Segmentation in English-Learning Infants , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[54]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Learning to Segment Speech Using Multiple Cues: A Connectionist Model , 1998 .

[55]  J. Vroomen,et al.  The Roles of Word Stress and Vowel Harmony in Speech Segmentation , 1998 .

[56]  Jeff Pressing,et al.  Asymmetric Cognitive Clock Structures in West African Rhythms , 1997 .

[57]  C. Drake,et al.  Tempo discrimination in infants , 1997 .

[58]  L. Trainor,et al.  The Acoustic Basis of Preferences for Infant-Directed Singing , 1997 .

[59]  S. Trehub,et al.  Mothers' and fathers' singing to infants. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[60]  R. Abrams,et al.  Fetal hearing: characterization of the stimulus and response. , 1996, Seminars in perinatology.

[61]  A. Lomax,et al.  American Ballads and Folk Songs , 1994 .

[62]  William P. Fifer,et al.  Two-day-olds prefer their native language , 1993 .

[63]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[64]  A. Fernald,et al.  A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants , 1989, Journal of Child Language.

[65]  S E Trehub,et al.  Infants' perception of rhythm: categorization of auditory sequences by temporal structure. , 1989, Canadian journal of psychology.

[66]  R N Aslin,et al.  The language environment of the young infant: implications for early perceptual development. , 1989, Canadian journal of psychology.

[67]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  A precursor of language acquisition in young infants , 1988, Cognition.

[68]  A. Decasper,et al.  Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns' perception of speech sounds , 1986 .

[69]  A. Fernald Four-Month-Old Infants Prefer to Listen to Motherese" , 1985 .

[70]  A. Decasper,et al.  Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices , 1980 .

[71]  D. Abercrombie,et al.  Elements of General Phonetics , 1967 .

[72]  Maud Karpeles Folk Songs of Europe , 1956, Journal of the International Folk Music Council.

[73]  S. Trehub,et al.  Expressive timing and dynamics in infant- directed and non-infant-directed singing , 2011 .

[74]  E. Hannon,et al.  Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture: a cross-cultural comparison. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[75]  Fred Cummins,et al.  Rhythm as entrainment: The case of synchronous speech , 2009, J. Phonetics.

[76]  Pilar Prieto,et al.  Rhythmic modification in child directed speech , 2009 .

[77]  Gábor P. Háden,et al.  UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Newborn infants detect the beat in music , 2009 .

[78]  P. Kuhl Is speech learning 'gated' by the social brain? , 2007, Developmental science.

[79]  E. Grabe,et al.  Durational variability in speech and the rhythm class hypothesis , 2005 .

[80]  J. Mehler,et al.  Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[81]  C. Palmer Music performance. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[82]  T. Tardif Nouns are not always learned before verbs : Evidence from Mandarin speakers' early vocabularies , 1996 .

[83]  P. Hepper An Examination of Fetal Learning Before and After Birth , 1991 .

[84]  R. Darlington,et al.  Regression and Linear Models , 1990 .

[85]  R. M. Dauer Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalyzed. , 1983 .

[86]  A. Prince,et al.  On stress and linguistic rhythm , 1977 .

[87]  E. Moore,et al.  Ballads and folk songs of the Southwest : more than 600 titles, melodies, and texts collected in Oklahoma , 1964 .

[88]  Ruth Crawford Seeger American Folk Songs for Children , 1948 .

[89]  A. Davison,et al.  140 folk-tunes : rote songs, grades I, II and III for school & home , 1944 .