Electrophysiological abnormalities of spatial attention in adults with autism during the gap overlap task

OBJECTIVE We evaluated event-related potentials (ERPs) elicited by attentional disengagement in individuals with autism. METHODS Sixteen adults with autism, 17 adults with mental retardation and 14 healthy adults participated in this study. We recorded the pre-saccade positive ERPs during the gap overlap task under which a peripheral stimulus was presented subsequent to a stimulus in the central visual field. Under the overlap condition, the central stimulus remained during the presentation of the peripheral stimulus and therefore participants need to disengage their attention intentionally in order to execute the saccade to the peripheral stimulus due to the preservation of the central stimulus. RESULTS The autism group elicited significantly higher pre-saccadic positivity during a period of 100-70 ms prior to the saccade onset than the other groups only under the overlap condition. The higher amplitude of pre-saccadic positivity in the overlap condition was significantly correlated with more severe clinical symptoms within the autism group. CONCLUSIONS These results demonstrate electrophysiological abnormalities of disengagement during visuospatial attention in adults with autism which cannot be attributed to their IQs. SIGNIFICANCE We suggest that adults with autism have deficits in attentional disengagement and the physiological substrates underlying deficits in autism and mental retardation are different.

[1]  A. Iwanami,et al.  Spatial attention in individuals with pervasive developmental disorders using the gap overlap task , 2004, Psychiatry Research.

[2]  H. R. Doig,et al.  Changes in the presaccadic spike potential with age. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[3]  H. Engeland,et al.  ERP differences among subtypes of pervasive developmental disorders , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[4]  Toshikazu Hasegawa,et al.  Reflexive orienting in response to eye gaze and an arrow in children with and without autism. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[5]  L. Schreibman,et al.  Stimulus overselectivity in autism: a review of research. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  James A. Russell,et al.  Autistic children's difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. , 1993 .

[7]  Susan E. Bryson,et al.  Visual-spatial orienting in autism , 1996, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[8]  Sarah Shomstein,et al.  Parietal Cortex Mediates Voluntary Control of Spatial and Nonspatial Auditory Attention , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[10]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  Jean-Louis Adrien,et al.  Hypersensitivity to acoustic change in children with autism: electrophysiological evidence of left frontal cortex dysfunctioning. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[12]  Chantal Kemner,et al.  Eye movements, visual attention, and autism: a saccadic reaction time study using the gap and overlap paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[13]  G. Csibra,et al.  Neural correlates of saccade planning in infants: a high-density ERP study. , 1998, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[14]  S Makeig,et al.  Event-related brain response abnormalities in autism: evidence for impaired cerebello-frontal spatial attention networks. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  E Courchesne,et al.  Effects of focused selective attention tasks on event-related potentials in autistic and normal individuals. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  J. Suckling,et al.  Mapping the brain in autism. A voxel-based MRI study of volumetric differences and intercorrelations in autism. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  Eric Courchesne,et al.  Contextual probability evaluation in autistic, receptive developmental language disorder, and control children: Event-related brain potential evidence , 1993, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[18]  N. Minshew,et al.  Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems but not cerebellar dysfunction in autism , 1999, Neurology.

[19]  H. Yamasue,et al.  Delayed automatic detection of change in speech sounds in adults with autism: A magnetoencephalographic study , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[20]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[21]  Matthew K Belmonte,et al.  Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[22]  Allen Allport,et al.  Visual attention , 1989 .

[23]  Eric Schopler,et al.  Use of the childhood autism rating scale with autistic adolescents and adults. , 1989, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[24]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[25]  E. Schopler,et al.  Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) , 1980, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[26]  Maha Adamo,et al.  The Functional Neuroanatomy of Spatial Attention in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2005, Developmental neuropsychology.

[27]  Eric Courchesne,et al.  Slowed orienting of covert visual-spatial attention in autism: Specific deficits associated with cerebellar and parietal abnormality , 1996, Development and Psychopathology.

[28]  Ruth Campbell,et al.  Does the perception of moving eyes trigger reflexive visual orienting in autism? , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[29]  L. Geffen,et al.  Event-related potentials in autistic and healthy children on an auditory choice reaction time task. , 1988, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[30]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Cortical development and saccade planning: The ontogeny of the spike potential , 2000, Neuroreport.

[31]  E Courchesne,et al.  Parietal lobe abnormalities detected with MR in patients with infantile autism. , 1993, AJR. American journal of roentgenology.

[32]  M. Corbetta,et al.  An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Orienting of Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  S. Zeki,et al.  The neurology of saccades and covert shifts in spatial attention: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Covert Visual Spatial Orienting and Saccades: Overlapping Neural Systems , 2000, NeuroImage.

[35]  W. Pritchard,et al.  Visual augmenting/reducing and P300 in autistic children , 1987, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[36]  F. Happé,et al.  Time to give up on a single explanation for autism , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  J. Russell,et al.  Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Dysfunctional attention in autistic savants. , 1993, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[39]  Gergely Csibra,et al.  Attention and oculomotor control: A high-density ERP study of the gap effect , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  M. Posner Foundations of cognitive science , 1989 .

[41]  Anneli Kylliäinen,et al.  Attention orienting by another's gaze direction in children with autism. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[42]  M. Atienza,et al.  Response latencies and event-related potentials during the gap paradigm using saccadic responses in human subjects. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[43]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Disengaging visual attention in the infant and adult , 1993 .

[44]  Carey D. Balaban,et al.  The human pre-saccadic spike potential: Influences of a visual target, saccade direction, electrode laterality and instructions to perform saccades , 1985, Brain Research.

[45]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Infants' fast saccades in the gap paradigm and development of visual attention☆ , 1997 .