What Neuroscience Can Tell about Intuitive Processes in the Context of Perceptual Discovery

According to the Oxford English Dictionary, intuition is the ability to understand or know something immediately, without conscious reasoning. Most people would agree that intuitive responses appear as ideas or feelings that subsequently guide our thoughts and behaviors. It is proposed that people continuously, without conscious attention, recognize patterns in the stream of sensations that impinge upon them. What exactly is being recognized is not clear yet, but we assume that people detect potential content based on only a few aspects of the input (i.e., the gist). The result is a vague perception of coherence which is not explicitly describable but instead embodied in a gut feeling or an initial guess, which subsequently biases thought and inquiry. To approach the nature of intuitive processes, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging when participants were working at a modified version of the Waterloo Gestalt Closure Task. Starting from our conceptualization that intuition involves an informed judgment in the context of discovery, we expected activation within the median orbito-frontal cortex (OFC), as this area receives input from all sensory modalities and has been shown to be crucially involved in emotionally driven decisions. Results from a direct contrast between intuitive and nonintuitive judgments, as well as from a parametric analysis, revealed the median OFC, the lateral portion of the amygdala, anterior insula, and ventral occipito-temporal regions to be activated. Based on these findings, we suggest our definition of intuition to be promising and a good starting point for future research on intuitive processes.

[1]  C. Cavada,et al.  The lateral geniculate nucleus projects to the inferior temporal cortex in the macaque monkey. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[2]  R. Davidson,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  Gabriele Lohmann,et al.  Bayesian second-level analysis of functional magnetic resonance images , 2003, NeuroImage.

[4]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Neural processes underlying intuitive coherence judgments as revealed by fMRI on a semantic judgment task , 2007, NeuroImage.

[5]  R. Elliott,et al.  Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  S. Petersen,et al.  Characterizing the Hemodynamic Response: Effects of Presentation Rate, Sampling Procedure, and the Possibility of Ordering Brain Activity Based on Relative Timing , 2000, NeuroImage.

[7]  G Lohmann,et al.  LIPSIA--a new software system for the evaluation of functional magnetic resonance images of the human brain. , 2001, Computerized medical imaging and graphics : the official journal of the Computerized Medical Imaging Society.

[8]  Costa Vakalopoulos,et al.  A theory of blindsight--the anatomy of the unconscious: a proposal for the koniocellular projections and intralaminar thalamus. , 2005, Medical hypotheses.

[9]  Vivien A. Casagrande,et al.  1 Parallel Visual Pathways in a Dynamic System , 2004 .

[10]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited—Again , 1995, NeuroImage.

[11]  Edward M. Bowden,et al.  New approaches to demystifying insight , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  R. Reid,et al.  The koniocellular pathway in primate vision. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[13]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[14]  S. Zeki,et al.  The chronoarchitecture of the cerebral cortex , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  A. Damasio The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[16]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[17]  Toralf Mildner,et al.  An Investigation of the Value of Spin-Echo-Based fMRI Using a Stroop Color–Word Matching Task and EPI at 3 T , 2002, NeuroImage.

[18]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  The human visual cortex. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  Geoffrey Schoenbaum,et al.  Rapid Associative Encoding in Basolateral Amygdala Depends on Connections with Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2005, Neuron.

[20]  G. Regehr,et al.  Intuition in the context of discovery , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[21]  E. Higgins,et al.  Regulatory Focus and Strategic Inclinations : Promotion and Prevention in Decision-Making , 1997 .

[22]  D G Norris,et al.  Reduced power multislice MDEFT imaging , 2000, Journal of magnetic resonance imaging : JMRI.

[23]  Timothy J. Andrews,et al.  Activity in the Fusiform Gyrus Predicts Conscious Perception of Rubin's Vase–Face Illusion , 2002, NeuroImage.

[24]  M. E. Corcoran,et al.  Susceptibility to Kindling and Neuronal Connections of the Anterior Claustrum , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  M. Posner The Brain and Emotion , 1999, Nature Medicine.

[26]  Kalanit Grill-Spector,et al.  The functional organization of the ventral visual pathway and its relationship to object recognition , 2003 .

[27]  A. B. Bonds,et al.  A comparison of koniocellular, magnocellular and parvocellular receptive field properties in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus) , 2001, The Journal of physiology.

[28]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[29]  C. Koch,et al.  What is the function of the claustrum? , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[30]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation , 1985, Neurology.

[31]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[32]  Joaquin M. Fuster,et al.  The prefrontal cortex of the primate: A synopsis , 2000, Psychobiology.

[33]  David W. Royal,et al.  Parallel Visual Pathways in a Dynamic System , 2003 .

[34]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Imaging at high magnetic fields: initial experiences at 4 T. , 1993, Magnetic resonance quarterly.

[35]  V. Casagrande,et al.  Synaptic and neurochemical characterization of parallel pathways to the cytochrome oxidase blobs of primate visual cortex , 1998, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[36]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. , 2002 .

[37]  E. Higgins,et al.  Beyond pleasure and pain. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[38]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Changing your mind: on the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature singletons. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  Karl J. Friston Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: A synthesis , 1994 .

[40]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Vase or face? A neural correlate of shape-selective grouping processes in the human brain , 2001, NeuroImage.

[41]  Edward M. Bowden,et al.  Neural Activity When People Solve Verbal Problems with Insight , 2004, PLoS biology.

[42]  R. Turner,et al.  Event-Related fMRI: Characterizing Differential Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[43]  E. Halgren,et al.  Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[44]  M. Roesch,et al.  Orbitofrontal Cortex, Associative Learning, and Expectancies , 2005, Neuron.

[45]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Detection of functional connectivity using temporal correlations in MR images , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[46]  Peter A. Bandettini,et al.  Detection versus Estimation in Event-Related fMRI: Choosing the Optimal Stimulus Timing , 2002, NeuroImage.

[47]  E. Rolls,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology , 2004, Progress in Neurobiology.

[48]  Peter Boesiger,et al.  Comparison of fMRI activation as measured with gradient- and spin-echo EPI during visual perception , 2005, NeuroImage.

[49]  R. Turner,et al.  Characterizing Evoked Hemodynamics with fMRI , 1995, NeuroImage.

[50]  S. Epstein Intuition from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. , 2008 .

[51]  M. Petrides,et al.  Differential activation of the human orbital, mid-ventrolateral, and mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during the processing of visual stimuli , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[52]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR , 1986 .

[53]  W. Nauta The problem of the frontal lobe: a reinterpretation. , 1971, Journal of psychiatric research.