A Virtual Reality Test Identifies the Visuospatial Strengths of Adolescents with Dyslexia

Research suggests that the deficits characterizing dyslexia may also be associated with superior visuospatial abilities. Other research suggests that superior visuospatial abilities of people with dyslexia may not have been so far identified because of the lack of appropriate tests of real-life spatial ability. A recent small-scale study found that visuospatial superiority was evident in men with dyslexia. This study assessed the visuospatial ability of adolescents with dyslexia in order to determine whether these adolescents performed better on a pseudo real-life visuospatial test than did their nondyslexic peers. Forty-two adolescents took part in the study. There was an equal numerical split between the experimental and control groups. The experimental group all had a diagnosis of dyslexia by an educational psychologist or specialist teacher. Visuospatial ability was assessed using the Recall of Designs and the Pattern Construction subtests from the British Ability Scales (2nd edition; BAS-11) together with a computer-generated virtual environment test. The assessments were administered in a counterbalanced order. Adolescents with dyslexia tended to perform less well than their nondyslexic peers on the BAS-11 tests; however, this difference was not statistically significant. For the computer-generated virtual environment test (pseudo real-life measure), statistically significant higher scores were achieved by the dyslexic group. These findings suggest that adolescents with dyslexia may exhibit superior visuospatial strengths on certain pseudo real-life tests of spatial ability. The usefulness of these findings is discussed in relation to possible implications for assessment and educational intervention programs for adolescents with dyslexia.

[1]  E. A. Attree,et al.  The specificity of memory enhancement during interaction with a virtual environment. , 1999, Memory.

[2]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Mental imagery and dyslexia: A deficit in processing multipart visual objects? , 1991, Brain and Language.

[3]  J. Beaumont Cerebral Lateralization: Biological Mechanisms, Associations, and Pathology , 1987 .

[4]  Sue Palmer,et al.  Phonological recoding deficit in working memory of dyslexic teenagers , 2000 .

[5]  A. Galaburda,et al.  Neocortical ectopias in BXSB mice: effects upon reference and working memory systems. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  A. Galaburda,et al.  Developmental dyslexia: four consecutive cases with cortical anomalies , 1995 .

[7]  Mahzarin R. Banaji,et al.  The Bankruptcy of Everyday Memory , 1989 .

[8]  Ellen Winner,et al.  Dyslexia linked to talent: Global visual-spatial ability , 2003, Brain and Language.

[9]  A Webster,et al.  Special educational needs code of practice , 1994 .

[10]  Ellen Winner,et al.  Dyslexia and Visual-Spatial Talents: Compensation vs Deficit Model , 2001, Brain and Language.

[11]  N. Geschwind,et al.  Developmental dyslexia: Four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies , 1985, Annals of neurology.

[12]  R. Riding,et al.  Cognitive Styles and Learning Strategies , 2013 .

[13]  J. Carroll Human Cognitive Abilities , 1993 .

[14]  R. Nicolson The dyslexia ecosystem. , 2002, Dyslexia.

[15]  Ingvar Lundberg,et al.  The prevalence of dyslexia among art students. , 2002, Dyslexia.

[16]  Uta Frith,et al.  Paradoxes in the definition of dyslexia , 1999 .

[17]  Edith B. LaFrance The gifted/dyslexic child: Characterizing and addressing strengths and weaknesses , 1997 .

[18]  Thomas G. West,et al.  In the Mind's Eye: Visual Thinkers, Gifted People with Dyslexia and Other Learning Difficulties, Computer Images and the Ironies of Creativity , 1997 .

[19]  John Everatt,et al.  An eye for the unusual: creative thinking in dyslexics , 1999 .

[20]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  The Neural Bases of Strategy and Skill in Sentence–Picture Verification , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.