Multiple tasks and neuroimaging modalities increase the likelihood of detecting covert awareness in patients with disorders of consciousness

Minimal or inconsistent behavioral responses to command make it challenging to accurately diagnose the level of awareness of a patient with a Disorder of consciousness (DOC). By identifying markers of mental imagery being covertly performed to command, functional neuroimaging (fMRI), electroencephalography (EEG) has shown that some of these patients are aware despite their lack of behavioral responsiveness. We report the findings of behavioral, fMRI, and EEG approaches to detecting command-following in a group of patients with DOC. From an initial sample of 14 patients, complete data across all tasks was obtained in six cases. Behavioral evaluations were performed with the Coma Recovery Scale—Revised. Both fMRI and EEG evaluations involved the completion of previously validated mental imagery tasks—i.e., motor imagery (EEG and fMRI) and spatial navigation imagery (fMRI). One patient exhibited statistically significant evidence of motor imagery in both the fMRI and EEG tasks, despite being unable to follow commands behaviorally. Two behaviorally non-responsive patients produced appropriate activation during the spatial navigation fMRI task. However, neither of these patients successfully completed the motor imagery tasks, likely due to specific motor area damage in at least one of these cases. A further patient demonstrated command following only in the EEG motor imagery task, and two patients did not demonstrate command following in any of the behavioral, EEG, or fMRI assessments. Due to the heterogeneity of etiology and pathology in this group, DOC patients vary in terms of their suitability for some forms of neuroimaging, the preservation of specific neural structures, and the cognitive resources that may be available to them. Assessments of a range of cognitive abilities supported by spatially-distinct brain regions and indexed by multiple neural signatures are therefore required in order to accurately characterize a patient's level of residual cognition and awareness.

[1]  J. Giacino,et al.  The minimally conscious state: Definition and diagnostic criteria , 2002, Neurology.

[2]  Srivas Chennu,et al.  Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state: a cohort study , 2011, The Lancet.

[3]  Moritz Grosse-Wentrup,et al.  Multisubject Learning for Common Spatial Patterns in Motor-Imagery BCI , 2011, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[4]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. , 2006, Science.

[5]  Steven Laureys,et al.  From unresponsive wakefulness to minimally conscious PLUS and functional locked-in syndromes: recent advances in our understanding of disorders of consciousness , 2011, Journal of Neurology.

[6]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[7]  Andrea Soddu,et al.  Lies, damned lies and diagnoses: Estimating the clinical utility of assessments of covert awareness in the vegetative state , 2014, Brain injury.

[8]  B. Jennett The Vegetative State: Medical Facts, Ethical and Legal Dilemmas , 2002 .

[9]  R. Munday,et al.  The Sensory Modality Assessment and Rehabilitation Technique (SMART): A Comprehensive and Integrated Assessment and Treatment Protocol for the Vegetative State and Minimally Responsive Patient , 1999 .

[10]  J. Giacino,et al.  The JFK coma recovery scale—revised , 2005, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[11]  J. Valach,et al.  [Disorders of consciousness]. , 1950, Slovensky lekar.

[12]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Detecting Activations in PET and fMRI: Levels of Inference and Power , 1996, NeuroImage.

[13]  Valeria Della-Maggiore,et al.  Functional Imaging Reveals Movement Preparatory Activity in the Vegetative State , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  Steven Laureys,et al.  The vegetative state , 2010, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[15]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Detecting Awareness in the Vegetative State , 2006, Science.

[16]  Rhodri Cusack,et al.  The Brain's Silent Messenger: Using Selective Attention to Decode Human Thought for Brain-Based Communication , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Athena Demertzi,et al.  Actigraphy assessments of circadian sleep-wake cycles in the Vegetative and Minimally Conscious States , 2013, BMC Medicine.

[18]  M. Boly,et al.  Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. , 2010, The New England journal of medicine.

[19]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Motor imagery activates primary sensorimotor area in humans , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.

[20]  M. Boly,et al.  Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: Clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment , 2009, BMC neurology.

[21]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Complexity and familiarity enhance single-trial detectability of imagined movements with electroencephalography , 2014, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[22]  G. B. Young,et al.  Practice Parameter: Prediction of outcome in comatose survivors after cardiopulmonary resuscitation (an evidence-based review) , 2006, Neurology.

[23]  M Lotze,et al.  The musician's brain: functional imaging of amateurs and professionals during performance and imagery , 2003, NeuroImage.

[24]  Wichian Sittiprapa,et al.  The Musician's Brain , 2012 .

[25]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Detecting awareness after severe brain injury , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  Adrian M Owen,et al.  Detecting consciousness: a unique role for neuroimaging. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.

[27]  S. Khoshbin,et al.  Evaluation of Postoperative Sharp Waveforms Through EEG and Magnetoencephalography , 2010, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[28]  Multi-Society Task Force on Pvs Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state (1). , 1994, The New England journal of medicine.

[29]  N. Schiff,et al.  Dissociations between behavioural and functional magnetic resonance imaging-based evaluations of cognitive function after brain injury. , 2011, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  Jing Luo,et al.  Sport expert's motor imagery: Functional imaging of professional motor skills and simple motor skills , 2010, Brain Research.

[31]  Multi-Society Task Force on Pvs Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state (2). , 1994, The New England journal of medicine.

[32]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Imagery of motor actions: differential effects of kinesthetic and visual-motor mode of imagery in single-trial EEG. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[33]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[34]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[35]  Marco Sarà,et al.  Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state – Authors' reply , 2012, The Lancet.

[36]  Montserrat Bernabeu,et al.  Combination of diffusion tensor and functional magnetic resonance imaging during recovery from the vegetative state , 2010, BMC neurology.

[37]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[38]  A. Owen,et al.  Making every word count for nonresponsive patients. , 2013, JAMA neurology.

[39]  Fang Sun,et al.  Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. , 2010, The New England journal of medicine.

[40]  E. Curran,et al.  Learning to control brain activity: A review of the production and control of EEG components for driving brain–computer interface (BCI) systems , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[41]  Steven Laureys,et al.  When thoughts become action: An fMRI paradigm to study volitional brain activity in non-communicative brain injured patients , 2007, NeuroImage.

[42]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  The Clinical Utility of fMRI for Identifying Covert Awareness in the Vegetative State: A Comparison of Sensitivity between 3T and 1.5T , 2014, PloS one.

[43]  I. Zijdewind,et al.  Corticospinal excitability during observation and imagery of simple and complex hand tasks: Implications for motor rehabilitation , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[44]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy underlying the clinical subcategorization of minimally conscious state patients , 2012, Journal of Neurology.

[45]  B. Jennett The vegetative state , 2002, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[46]  Jonathan D. Victor,et al.  Determination of awareness in patients with severe brain injury using EEG power spectral analysis , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[47]  M. Stokes,et al.  Cognitive tasks for driving a brain-computer interfacing system: a pilot study , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.

[48]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Detecting Awareness in the Vegetative State: Electroencephalographic Evidence for Attempted Movements to Command , 2012, PloS one.

[49]  N. Childs,et al.  Accuracy of diagnosis of persistent vegetative state , 1993, Neurology.