A Regret-Induced Status Quo Bias

A suboptimal bias toward accepting the status quo option in decision-making is well established behaviorally, but the underlying neural mechanisms are less clear. Behavioral evidence suggests the emotion of regret is higher when errors arise from rejection rather than acceptance of a status quo option. Such asymmetry in the genesis of regret might drive the status quo bias on subsequent decisions, if indeed erroneous status quo rejections have a greater neuronal impact than erroneous status quo acceptances. To test this, we acquired human fMRI data during a difficult perceptual decision task that incorporated a trial-to-trial intrinsic status quo option, with explicit signaling of outcomes (error or correct). Behaviorally, experienced regret was higher after an erroneous status quo rejection compared with acceptance. Anterior insula and medial prefrontal cortex showed increased blood oxygenation level-dependent signal after such status quo rejection errors. In line with our hypothesis, a similar pattern of signal change predicted acceptance of the status quo on a subsequent trial. Thus, our data link a regret-induced status quo bias to error-related activity on the preceding trial.

[1]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Avoiding another mistake: Error and posterror neural activity associated with adaptive posterror behavior change , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  R. Turner,et al.  Combined correction for geometric distortion and its interaction with head movement in fMRI , 2002 .

[4]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Modelling Geometric Deformations in Epi Time Series , 2022 .

[5]  Gregory S. Berns,et al.  Neurobiological regret and rejoice functions for aversive outcomes , 2008, NeuroImage.

[6]  Maurice E. Schweitzer,et al.  DISENTANGLING STATUS QUO AND OMISSION EFFECTS: AN EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS , 1994 .

[7]  Marcel Zeelenberg,et al.  A Theory of Regret Regulation 1.0 , 2007 .

[8]  J. Baron,et al.  Status-quo and omission biases , 1992 .

[9]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Unified segmentation , 2005, NeuroImage.

[11]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  C. Anderson The Psychology of Doing Nothing: Forms of Decision Avoidance Result from Reason and Emotion , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  Behavioral Law and Economics: Reluctance to Vaccinate: Omission Bias and Ambiguity , 1990 .

[14]  P. McLeod,et al.  Why good thoughts block better ones: The mechanism of the pernicious Einstellung (set) effect , 2008, Cognition.

[15]  S. Quartz,et al.  Human Insula Activation Reflects Risk Prediction Errors As Well As Risk , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  J. Landman Regret and Elation Following Action and Inaction , 1987 .

[17]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. , 1999, Science.

[18]  H. Critchley,et al.  Fear Conditioning in Humans The Influence of Awareness and Autonomic Arousal on Functional Neuroanatomy , 2002, Neuron.

[19]  G. Glover,et al.  Error‐related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[20]  R Turner,et al.  Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T , 2004, NeuroImage.

[21]  Thane S. Pittman,et al.  The consequences of doing nothing: Inaction inertia as avoidance of anticipated counterfactual regret. , 1998 .

[22]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  The Mechanism of the Einstellung (Set) Effect , 2010 .

[23]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  A. Tenbrunsel,et al.  Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes , 2013 .

[25]  Stephen M Fleming,et al.  Overcoming status quo bias in the human brain , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Modelling event-related skin conductance responses , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[27]  William Samuelson,et al.  Status quo bias in decision making , 1988 .

[28]  M. Zeelenberg,et al.  Regret in Decision Making , 2002 .

[29]  R. Pieters,et al.  The inaction effect in the psychology of regret. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[31]  Norbert Kathmann,et al.  Neural correlates of error awareness , 2007, NeuroImage.

[32]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  A Role for the Striatum in Regret-related Choice Repetition , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  Thane S. Pittman,et al.  Inaction Inertia in the Stock Market1 , 2004 .

[34]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Involvement of the Orbitofrontal Cortex in the Experience of Regret , 2004, Science.

[35]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  Reference Points and Omission Bias , 1994 .

[36]  B. Gold,et al.  Functional Dissociation in Frontal and Striatal Areas for Processing of Positive and Negative Reward Information , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  E. Loftus,et al.  Are Actions Regretted More Than Inactions? , 1999, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[38]  Stephan F. Taylor,et al.  Decision-related loss: Regret and disappointment , 2009, NeuroImage.

[39]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives , 1986 .

[40]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[41]  M. Zeelenberg,et al.  Regret in Repeat Purchase versus Switching Decisions: The Attenuating Role of Decision Justifiability , 2002 .

[42]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neural Activity in the Human Brain Relating to Uncertainty and Arousal during Anticipation , 2001, Neuron.

[43]  Giorgio Coricelli,et al.  Response to Comment on "The Involvement of the Orbitofrontal Cortex in the Experience of Regret" , 2005, Science.

[44]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Neuroimaging of Performance Monitoring: Error Detection and Beyond , 2004, Cortex.

[45]  R. Dolan,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Social Neuroscience Amygdala Involvement in Self-blame Regret Amygdala Involvement in Self-blame Regret Amygdala and Self-blame Regret , 2022 .

[46]  T. Robbins,et al.  Differential effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging: methods and applications. , 2007, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[48]  I. Liberzon,et al.  The Neurocircuitry of Fear, Stress, and Anxiety Disorders , 2011, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[49]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modeling of spontaneous fluctuations in skin conductance , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[50]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Neural mechanisms involved in error processing: A comparison of errors made with and without awareness , 2005, NeuroImage.

[51]  J. Baron,et al.  Omission and commission in judgment and choice , 1991 .

[52]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Time-series analysis for rapid event-related skin conductance responses , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[53]  Camelia M. Kuhnen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Financial Risk Taking , 2005, Neuron.

[54]  H. Critchley,et al.  A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  J. Baron,et al.  Outcome Knowledge, Regret, and Omission Bias , 1995 .

[56]  Evan M. Gordon,et al.  Neural Signatures of Economic Preferences for Risk and Ambiguity , 2006, Neuron.

[57]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Imaging at high magnetic fields: initial experiences at 4 T. , 1993, Magnetic resonance quarterly.

[58]  M. Paulus,et al.  An Insular View of Anxiety , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[59]  V. Mittal,et al.  Regret: A Model of its Antecedents and Consequences in Consumer Decision Making , 2000 .

[60]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The role of cingulate cortex in the detection of errors with and without awareness: a high‐density electrical mapping study , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[61]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The Anterior Cingulate and Error Avoidance , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  Thane S. Pittman,et al.  Product Aversion Following a Missed Opportunity: Price Contrast or Avoidance of Anticipated Regret? , 2001 .

[63]  J H Kordes-de Vaal,et al.  Intention and the omission bias: omissions perceived as nondecisions. , 1996, Acta psychologica.

[64]  Corianne Rogalsky,et al.  Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism , 2003, NeuroImage.

[65]  J. H. Kordes de Vaal,et al.  Intention and the omission bias: Omissions perceived as nondecisions , 1996 .

[66]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Dissociating Valence of Outcome from Behavioral Control in Human Orbital and Ventral Prefrontal Cortices , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  Robert Turner,et al.  Image Distortion Correction in fMRI: A Quantitative Evaluation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[68]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: A whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T , 2006, NeuroImage.

[69]  A. Tversky,et al.  The simulation heuristic , 1982 .

[70]  A. Craig How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.