Monge: The Verriest Lecture, Lyon, July 2005

In 1789, when neither the physical basis of hue nor the retinal basis of color perception was established, the mathematician Gaspard Monte stated firmly that our color perceptions do not depend on the absolute value of the physical variable, but are influenced by the context and in particular by our estimate of the illuminant. He used this insight to explain color contrast effects and the Paradox of Monge (the desaturation of red objects seen through a red filter). He proposed that we can estimate the chromaticity of the illuminant in any scene because all surfaces reflect to us varying mixtures of (i) the body color and (ii) a specular component that represents the illuminant. He also realized that white objects have a special property: Provided that they are illuminated by a single illuminant, such objects exhibit no variation in chromaticity across their surface. Thus at least one of the unique hues exists as an external reference on which observers can agree. It is suggested that other unique hues may also have a basis in the external world.

[1]  J. Bosten,et al.  Multidimensional scaling reveals a color dimension unique to ‘color-deficient’ observers , 2005, Current Biology.

[2]  H E Smithson,et al.  Sensory, computational and cognitive components of human colour constancy , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[3]  H. Shinoda,et al.  Recognized visual space of illumination: A new account of center-surround simultaneous color contrast , 2004 .

[4]  MF Lyon X‐chromosome inactivation and human genetic disease , 2002, Acta paediatrica (Oslo, Norway : 1992). Supplement.

[5]  Steven K. Shevell,et al.  Color appearance with sparse chromatic context , 1995, Vision Research.

[6]  M. Crosland In the Shadow of Lavoisier: The "Annales De Chimie" and the Establishment of a New Science , 1994 .

[7]  J. Werner,et al.  The invariance of unique white; a possible implication for normalizing cone action spectra , 1991, Vision Research.

[8]  J. Mollon "Tho' she kneel'd in that place where they grew..." The uses and origins of primate colour vision. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[9]  M D'Zmura,et al.  Mechanisms of color constancy. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[10]  H C Lee,et al.  Method for computing the scene-illuminant chromaticity from specular highlights. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[11]  P. Lennie,et al.  Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  D. W. Heeley,et al.  Cardinal directions of color space , 1982, Vision Research.

[13]  Colin Jones Charles Coulston Gillispie, Science and polity in France at the end of the Old Regime, Princeton University Press, 1981, pp. xxii, 601, £22.30. , 1982, Medical History.

[14]  S. Zeki The representation of colours in the cerebral cortex , 1980, Nature.

[15]  R. M. Boynton,et al.  Chromaticity diagram showing cone excitation by stimuli of equal luminance. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[16]  Audrey Tarrant,et al.  Color in Business, Science and Industry , 1976 .

[17]  郁夫 藤村,et al.  色対比と Color Appearance , 1970 .

[18]  G. Brindley The physiology of the retina and visual pathways , 1962 .

[19]  G. Brindley Physiology of the Retina and the Visual Pathway , 1960 .

[20]  H. Vries,et al.  The heredity of the relative numbers of red and green receptors in the human eye , 1949, Genetica.

[21]  Forrest L. Dimmick,et al.  The Spectral Location of Psychologically Unique Yellow, Green, and Blue , 1939 .

[22]  O. Reiser,et al.  Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .

[23]  D. L. Macadam Maximum Visual Efficiency of Colored Materials , 1935 .

[24]  Yannick Roman MUSEUM NATIONAL D'HISTOIRE NATURELLE , 2008 .

[25]  Josef W. Konvitz,et al.  Science and Polity in France: The Revolutionary and Napoleonic Years , 2005 .

[26]  ßjss,et al.  Kaiserliche Akademie der Wissenschaften in Wien , 2005, Österreichische botanische Zeitschrift.

[27]  A. Hurlbert,et al.  Color contrast: a contributory mechanism to color constancy. , 2004, Progress in brain research.

[28]  J. Mollon 1 – The Origins of Modern Color Science , 2003 .

[29]  F. Pairault Gaspard Monge : le fondateur de polytechnique , 2000 .

[30]  John D. Mollon,et al.  Unique hues in heterozygotes for protan and deutan deficiencies , 1997 .

[31]  John D. Mollon Chevreul et sa théorie de la vision dans le cadre du XIXe siècle , 1997 .

[32]  G. Roque,et al.  Michel-Eugène Chevreul : Un savant, des couleurs ! , 1997 .

[33]  Raoul Chevreul La vie et l'œuvre de Michel-Eugène Chevreul , 1997 .

[34]  L. Cosmides,et al.  The Adapted mind : evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture , 1992 .

[35]  Roger N. Shepard,et al.  The perceptual organization of colors: An adaptation to regularities of the terrestrial world? , 1992 .

[36]  C. Cicerone COLOR APPEARANCE AND THE CONE MOSAIC IN TRICHROMACY AND DICHROMACY , 1990 .

[37]  O. Guericke Esperimenta [i.e. Experimenta] nova (ut vocantur) magdeburgica de vacuo spatio , 1967 .

[38]  P. Aubry Monge : le savant ami de Napoléon Bonaparte 1746-1818 , 1954 .

[39]  Eugène Chevreul,et al.  De la loi du contraste simultané des couleurs et de l'assortiment des objets colorés , 1839 .

[40]  E. Chevreul Mémoire sur l'influence que deux couleurs peuvent avoir l'une sur l'autre quand on les voit simultanément : lu à l'Académie des sciences, le 7 avril 1828 / par M. Chevreul , 2022 .

[41]  Thomas Young,et al.  I. The Bakerian Lecture. Experiments and calculations relative to physical optics , 1804, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.