Individual differences in non-symbolic numerical abilities predict mathematical achievements but contradict ATOM

BackgroundA significant debate surrounds the nature of the cognitive mechanisms involved in non-symbolic number estimation. Several studies have suggested the existence of the same cognitive system for estimation of time, space, and number, called “a theory of magnitude” (ATOM). In addition, researchers have proposed the theory that non-symbolic number abilities might support our mathematical skills. Despite the large number of studies carried out, no firm conclusions can be drawn on either topic.MethodsIn the present study, we correlated the performance of adults on non-symbolic magnitude estimations and symbolic numerical tasks. Non-symbolic magnitude abilities were assessed by asking participants to estimate which auditory tone lasted longer (time), which line was longer (space), and which group of dots was more numerous (number). To assess symbolic numerical abilities, participants were required to perform mental calculations and mathematical reasoning.ResultsWe found a positive correlation between non-symbolic and symbolic numerical abilities. On the other hand, no correlation was found among non-symbolic estimations of time, space, and number.ConclusionsOur study supports the idea that mathematical abilities rely on rudimentary numerical skills that predate verbal language. By contrast, the lack of correlation among non-symbolic estimations of time, space, and number is incompatible with the idea that these magnitudes are entirely processed by the same cognitive system.

[1]  Guilherme Wood,et al.  A developmental fMRI study of nonsymbolic numerical and spatial processing , 2008, Cortex.

[2]  Hilla Peretz,et al.  Ju n 20 03 Schrödinger ’ s Cat : The rules of engagement , 2003 .

[3]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[4]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  The Role of Visual Information in Numerosity Estimation , 2012, PloS one.

[5]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[6]  Michael J Beran,et al.  Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) respond to nonvisible sets after one-by-one addition and removal of items. , 2004, Journal of comparative psychology.

[7]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[8]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Through Neural Stimulation to Behavior Manipulation: A Novel Method for Analyzing Dynamical Cognitive Models , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[9]  Fei Xu,et al.  Number discrimination in 10-month-old infants , 2007 .

[10]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Representation of number in the brain. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Origins of Number Sense , 2003, Psychological science.

[12]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Core information processing deficits in developmental dyscalculia and low numeracy. , 2008, Developmental science.

[13]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[14]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments , 2004, Neuron.

[15]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[16]  M. Noël,et al.  Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing , 2007, Cognition.

[17]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  Core knowledge. , 2007, Developmental science.

[18]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Linear Numerical-Magnitude Representations Aid Children’s Memory for Numbers , 2010, Psychological science.

[19]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  General Magnitude Representation in Human Infants , 2010, Psychological science.

[20]  E. Spelke,et al.  Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.

[22]  Wolfgang Grodd,et al.  Comparison of quantities: core and format-dependent regions as revealed by fMRI. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  M. Pesenti,et al.  Numerosity-duration interference: a Stroop experiment. , 2006, Acta psychologica.

[24]  Orly Rubinsten,et al.  Symbolic and non symbolic numerical representation in adults with and without developmental dyscalculia , 2012, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[25]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Malleability of the approximate number system: effects of feedback and training , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[27]  Pavlos C Filippopoulos,et al.  Interference between auditory and visual duration judgements suggests a common code for time , 2013, Psychological research.

[28]  S. Goldstone,et al.  Auditory-Visual Differences in Human Temporal Judgment , 1972, Perceptual and motor skills.

[29]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Basic Math in Monkeys and College Students , 2007, PLoS biology.

[30]  Frank H. Durgin,et al.  Texture density adaptation and the perceived numerosity and distribution of texture. , 1995 .

[31]  Stefan Golaszewski,et al.  Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects in a numerical Stroop task: an event-related fMRI study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[32]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[33]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[34]  L. Feigenson,et al.  Preschoolers' Precision of the Approximate Number System Predicts Later School Mathematics Performance , 2011, PloS one.

[35]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Does Subitizing Reflect Numerical Estimation? , 2022 .

[36]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Links Between the Intuitive Sense of Number and Formal Mathematics Ability. , 2013, Child development perspectives.

[37]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Evidence for Two Numerical Systems That Are Similar in Humans and Guppies , 2012, PloS one.

[38]  C. Caltagirone,et al.  Perceiving numbers alters time perception , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[39]  J. B.,et al.  The Power of Numerical Discrimination , 1871, Nature.

[40]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[41]  Silke M. Göbel,et al.  Impact of High Mathematics Education on the Number Sense , 2012, PloS one.

[42]  D. J. Bobko,et al.  The Perception of Brief Temporal Intervals: Power Functions for Auditory and Visual Stimulus Intervals , 1977, Perception.

[43]  Christian Agrillo,et al.  Once upon a time there was complex numerical estimation , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[44]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Numerical ordering ability mediates the relation between number-sense and arithmetic competence , 2011, Cognition.

[45]  T. Lehtimäki,et al.  Behavioral and Brain Functions , 2008 .

[46]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Intuitive sense of number correlates with math scores on college-entrance examination. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[47]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Time and numerosity estimation are independent: Behavioral evidence for two different systems using a conflict paradigm , 2010, Cognitive neuroscience.

[48]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[49]  V. Walsh,et al.  The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[50]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling , 2010, Cognition.

[51]  M. Beran,et al.  Putting the elephant back in the herd: elephant relative quantity judgments match those of other species , 2012, Animal Cognition.

[52]  Do Social Conditions Affect Capuchin Monkeys’ (Cebus apella) Choices in a Quantity Judgment Task? , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[53]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Foundational numerical capacities and the origins of dyscalculia , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  A. Bisazza,et al.  Large Number Discrimination by Mosquitofish , 2010, PloS one.

[55]  J. Ross Visual Discrimination of Number without Counting , 2003, Perception.

[56]  Marinella Cappelletti,et al.  Numbers and time doubly dissociate , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  Manuela Piazza,et al.  Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[59]  O. Güntürkün,et al.  Evidence for a Numerosity Category that is Based on Abstract Qualities of “Few” vs. “Many” in the Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[60]  Sheng He,et al.  Larger stimuli are judged to last longer. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[61]  S. Goldstone,et al.  Studies of Auditory-Visual Differences in Human Time Judgment: 1. Sounds are Judged Longer than Lights , 1974, Perceptual and motor skills.

[62]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[63]  Simone Cutini,et al.  Subitizing and visual short-term memory in human and non-human species: a common shared system? , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[64]  Alexis Garland,et al.  Memory for Multiple Cache Locations and Prey Quantities in a Food-Hoarding Songbird , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[65]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.