‘Bodily precision’: a predictive coding account of individual differences in interoceptive accuracy

Individuals differ in their awareness of afferent information from within their bodies, which is typically assessed by a heartbeat perception measure of ‘interoceptive accuracy’ (IAcc). Neural and behavioural correlates of this trait have been investigated, but a theoretical explanation has yet to be presented. Building on recent models that describe interoception within the free energy/predictive coding framework, this paper applies similar principles to IAcc, proposing that individual differences in IAcc depend on ‘precision’ in interoceptive systems, i.e. the relative weight accorded to ‘prior’ representations and ‘prediction errors’ (that part of incoming interoceptive sensation not accounted for by priors), at various levels within the cortical hierarchy and between modalities. Attention has the effect of optimizing precision both within and between sensory modalities. Our central assumption is that people with high IAcc are able, with attention, to prioritize interoception over other sensory modalities and can thus adjust the relative precision of their interoceptive priors and prediction errors, where appropriate, given their personal history. This characterization explains key findings within the interoception literature; links results previously seen as unrelated or contradictory; and may have important implications for understanding cognitive, behavioural and psychopathological consequences of both high and low interoceptive awareness. This article is part of the themed issue ‘Interoception beyond homeostasis: affect, cognition and mental health’.

[1]  W. Whitehead,et al.  Perception of gastric contractions and self-control of gastric motility. , 1980, Psychophysiology.

[2]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. , 1999 .

[3]  M. Antony,et al.  Heartbeat perception in panic disorder: a reanalysis. , 2000, Behaviour research and therapy.

[4]  A. Öhman,et al.  Nonconscious Fear Conditioning, Visceral Perception, and the Development of Gut Feelings , 2001, Psychological science.

[5]  O. Pollatos,et al.  Accuracy of heartbeat perception is reflected in the amplitude of the heartbeat-evoked brain potential. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[6]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  O. Pollatos,et al.  Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: A dipole source localization study , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[8]  R. Schandry,et al.  Interoceptive sensitivity and physical effort: implications for the self-control of physical load in everyday life. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[9]  Hui Yuan 袁辉,et al.  Effect of heartbeat perception on heartbeat evoked potential waves , 2007, Neuroscience Bulletin.

[10]  O. Pollatos,et al.  Neural systems connecting interoceptive awareness and feelings , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[11]  O. Pollatos,et al.  Interoceptive sensitivity and emotion processing: an EEG study. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[12]  Beate M Herbert,et al.  Heart rate response after emotional picture presentation is modulated by interoceptive awareness. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[13]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding explains binocular rivalry: An epistemological review , 2008, Cognition.

[14]  S. Quartz,et al.  Human Insula Activation Reflects Risk Prediction Errors As Well As Risk , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Karl J. Friston The free-energy principle: a rough guide to the brain? , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  A. Craig,et al.  How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  Rhodri Cusack,et al.  Listening to Your Heart , 2010, Psychological science.

[18]  K. Domschke,et al.  Interoceptive sensitivity in anxiety and anxiety disorders: an overview and integration of neurobiological findings. , 2010, Clinical psychology review.

[19]  R. Schandry,et al.  Implicit memory for emotional words is modulated by cardiac perception , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[20]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Attention, Uncertainty, and Free-Energy , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[21]  V. Menon,et al.  Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[22]  M. Paulus,et al.  Interoception in anxiety and depression , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[23]  Satoshi Umeda,et al.  Association between interoception and empathy: evidence from heartbeat-evoked brain potential. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[24]  O. Pollatos,et al.  On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? , 2011, Journal of personality.

[25]  R. Raedt,et al.  Interoceptive awareness and unaware fear conditioning: Are subliminal conditioning effects influenced by the manipulation of visceral self-perception? , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[26]  Manos Tsakiris,et al.  Just a heartbeat away from one's body: interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  G. Moseley,et al.  The rubber hand illusion increases histamine reactivity in the real arm , 2011, Current Biology.

[28]  Ana B. Chica,et al.  Attentional Routes to Conscious Perception , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[29]  H. Critchley,et al.  An Interoceptive Predictive Coding Model of Conscious Presence , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[30]  A. Woods,et al.  Context Modulates the Contribution of Time and Space in Causal Inference , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[31]  J. Hohwy Attention and Conscious Perception in the Hypothesis Testing Brain , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[32]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A Bayesian account of ‘hysteria’ , 2012, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  B. Egloff,et al.  Is interoceptive awareness really altered in somatoform disorders? Testing competing theories with two paradigms of heartbeat perception. , 2012, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[34]  O. Pollatos,et al.  Interoception across Modalities: On the Relationship between Cardiac Awareness and the Sensitivity for Gastric Functions , 2012, PloS one.

[35]  H. Critchley,et al.  Visceral Influences on Brain and Behavior , 2013, Neuron.

[36]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness , 2013, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[37]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Attention Sharpens the Distinction between Expected and Unexpected Percepts in the Visual Brain , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  N. Farb,et al.  Attentional modulation of primary interoceptive and exteroceptive cortices. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  Beyond the reward principle , 2013 .

[40]  A. Fotopoulou Beyond the Reward Principle: Consciousness as Precision Seeking , 2013 .

[41]  M. Tsakiris,et al.  More of myself: Manipulating interoceptive awareness by heightened attention to bodily and narrative aspects of the self , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Active inference, sensory attenuation and illusions , 2013, Cognitive Processing.

[43]  Klaus Gramann,et al.  On the embodiment of emotion regulation: interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[44]  R. Goebel,et al.  Local Discriminability Determines the Strength of Holistic Processing for Faces in the Fusiform Face Area , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[45]  V. Gallese,et al.  Closing the Gap between the Inside and the Outside: Interoceptive Sensitivity and Social Distances , 2013, PloS one.

[46]  Mateus Joffily,et al.  Emotional Valence and the Free-Energy Principle , 2013, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[47]  J. Daunizeau,et al.  Neurocomputational account of how the human brain decides when to have a break , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[48]  H. Critchley,et al.  Multisensory integration across exteroceptive and interoceptive domains modulates self-experience in the rubber-hand illusion , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  E. Seifritz,et al.  Interoceptive awareness enhances neural activity during empathy , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[50]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Computational Anatomy of Psychosis , 2013, Front. Psychiatry.

[51]  A. Seth Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  J. Katayama,et al.  Neural correlates of error processing reflect individual differences in interoceptive sensitivity. , 2014, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[53]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Heartfelt imitation: High interoceptive awareness is linked to greater automatic imitation , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Malgorzata Marjanska,et al.  GABA in the insula — a predictor of the neural response to interoceptive awareness , 2014, NeuroImage.

[55]  Erich Seifritz,et al.  The association of interoceptive awareness and alexithymia with neurotransmitter concentrations in insula and anterior cingulate. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[56]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Bayesian inference, predictive coding and delusions , 2014 .

[57]  M. Tsakiris,et al.  The free-energy self: A predictive coding account of self-recognition , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[58]  Giovanni Pezzulo,et al.  Why do you fear the bogeyman? An embodied predictive coding model of perceptual inference , 2013, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[59]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 2014 .

[60]  Thomas H. B. FitzGerald,et al.  Interoceptive inference: homeostasis and decision-making , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[61]  M. Husain,et al.  The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[62]  C. Ring,et al.  Effects of heartbeat feedback on beliefs about heart rate and heartbeat counting: A cautionary tale about interoceptive awareness , 2015, Biological Psychology.

[63]  N. Farb,et al.  Interoception, contemplative practice, and health , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[64]  G. Lohmann,et al.  Interoceptive awareness changes the posterior insula functional connectivity profile , 2015, Brain Structure and Function.

[65]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Optimal inference with suboptimal models: Addiction and active Bayesian inference , 2015, Medical hypotheses.

[66]  Justin S. Feinstein,et al.  Heart evoked potential triggers brain responses to natural affective scenes: A preliminary study , 2015, Autonomic Neuroscience.

[67]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Interoceptive predictions in the brain , 2015, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[68]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Active Inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural control , 2015, Progress in Neurobiology.

[69]  O. Pollatos,et al.  Perceiving one's body shapes empathy , 2015, Physiology & Behavior.

[70]  M. Husain,et al.  Characterization of reward and effort mechanisms in apathy , 2015, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[71]  H. Critchley,et al.  Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness , 2015, Biological Psychology.

[72]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  An active inference theory of allostasis and interoception in depression , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.