Further evidence for the role of mode-independent short-term associations in spatial Simon effects

We examined the conditions under which short-term associations between stimuli and responses can produce spatial Simon effects. On location-relevant trials, participants gave neutral responses (i.e., they uttered the nonsense syllable “bee” or “boo”) on the basis of whether the presented word had the meaning of “left” or “right.” On location-irrelevant trials, they gave the same responses on the basis of the color of left and right squares. Performance on the location-irrelevant trials was affected by the match between the irrelevant location information and the location to which the correct response was assigned on the location-relevant trials. Experiment 1 showed that this extrinsic Simon effect was found only when the words on the location-relevant trials came from two different languages. In Experiment 2, we found an extrinsic Simon effect even when participants only received instructions about how to respond on location-relevant trials but no such trials were actually presented. Our findings suggest that task demands determine whether short-term associations are mode specific or mode independent and confirm that such associations can be set up as the result of instructions only.

[1]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .

[2]  R. Proctor,et al.  The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[3]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Cross-modal re-mapping influences the Simon effect , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[4]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  R. Proctor,et al.  Mixing incompatibly mapped location-relevant trials with location-irrelevant trials: effects of stimulus mode on the reverse Simon effect , 2000, Psychological research.

[6]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[7]  Jan De Houwer Spatial Simon effects with nonspatial responses. , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  Jan De Houwer,et al.  A structural analysis of indirect measures of attitudes , 2003 .

[9]  B. Hommel Toward an action-concept model of stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .

[10]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[11]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[12]  J. R. Simon The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .

[13]  S Kornblum,et al.  The effects of stimulus-response mapping and irrelevant stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus overlap in four-choice Stroop tasks with single-carrier stimuli. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  M. Zorzi,et al.  The role of long-term-memory and short-term-memory links in the Simon effect. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  R. Proctor,et al.  Processing irrelevant location information: Practice and transfer effects in choice-reaction tasks , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[16]  P. Barber,et al.  The relevance of salience: Towards an activational account of irrelevant stimulus-response compatibility effects , 1997 .

[17]  W. Prinz,et al.  Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .

[18]  J. de Houwer The Extrinsic Affective Simon Task. , 2003, Experimental psychology.

[19]  M. Zorzi,et al.  A computational model of the Simon effect , 1995, Psychological research.