Striatal lesions in the mouse disrupt acquisition and retention, but not implicit learning, in the SILT procedural motor learning task

People with Huntington's disease (HD) have been found to have an implicit learning deficit whereby they are typically unable to detect repeated sequences embedded within randomly presented stimuli. The operant serial implicit learning task (SILT) was designed to probe animal models of HD for implicit learning deficits using the 9-hole box apparatus. The present study used mice to determine whether "early" striatal lesions would prevent SILT acquisition and to confirm previous findings that post-training "late lesions" would impair the retention of task performance. The SILT is a two-phase task whereby an initial stimulus light (S1) presentation was presented in one of five possible locations. A correct nose-poke response to the S1 resulted in this light being extinguished and a second, apparently random light presentation (S2). A correct nose-poke to S2 resulted in a reward. Within the apparently random stimulus light presentations, a predictable S1/S2 combination was embedded. Both lesion groups ("early" pre-acquisition and "late" post-acquisition lesions) demonstrated increased reaction times to S1, with the late-lesion group also recording reduced task accuracy when compared with the sham control group. The early-lesion group also demonstrated increased response latencies for the S2 stimuli during task acquisition, this was also true for task retention in the late-lesion group. No difference between the control group and early-lesion group was found for the S2 response accuracy during the acquisition period. After the lesioning of the late-lesion group, both lesion groups demonstrated reduced accuracy to the S2 stimuli as the control group improved their performance throughout the test period, while the accuracy of both lesion groups remained stable at a lower performance level. All three experimental groups were able to utilize the embedded predictable information. The present data suggest that the striatum is important for the acquisition and retention of motor learning tasks, but does not play a role in the learning of implicit information.

[1]  M. Nissen,et al.  Procedural learning is impaired in Huntington's disease: Evidence from the serial reaction time task , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  J. Ashe,et al.  Selective impairments in implicit learning in Parkinson's disease , 2007, Brain Research.

[3]  D. Salmon,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: a comparison of Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's disease patients , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  Carolee J Winstein,et al.  Providing explicit information disrupts implicit motor learning after basal ganglia stroke. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[5]  Patrik Brundin,et al.  Huntington's disease: a synaptopathy? , 2003, Trends in molecular medicine.

[6]  R. S. Williams,et al.  Evidence for neuronal degeneration and dendritic plasticity in cortical pyramidal neurons of Huntington's disease , 1993, Neurology.

[7]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding. A PET study. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  S. Dunnett,et al.  An operant serial implicit learning task (SILT) in rats: Task acquisition, performance and the effects of striatal lesions , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[9]  M. Paulus,et al.  Deficit in schizophrenia to recruit the striatum in implicit learning: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation , 2006, Schizophrenia Research.

[10]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Intact mirror-tracing and impaired rotary-pursuit skill learning in patients with Huntington's disease: evidence for dissociable memory systems in skill learning. , 1997 .

[11]  T. Robbins,et al.  Executive and mnemonic functions in early Huntington's disease. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  G. Sobue,et al.  An autopsy case with clinically and molecular genetically diagnosed Huntington's disease with only minimal non-specific neuropathological findings. , 2000, Clinical neuropathology.

[13]  R. Ferrante,et al.  Neuropathological Classification of Huntington's Disease , 1985, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[14]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Separate neural substrates for skill learning and performance in the ventral and dorsal striatum , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Visuomotor Skill Learning in Human Subjects Examined with Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[16]  Tao Liu,et al.  Impaired predictive motor timing in patients with cerebellar disorders , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Terry L. Jernigan,et al.  Cerebral structure on MRI, part II: Specific changes in Alzheimer's and Huntington's diseases , 1991, Biological Psychiatry.

[18]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  In vivo evidence of cerebellar atrophy and cerebral white matter loss in Huntington disease , 2004, Neurology.

[19]  S. Dunnett,et al.  Implicit learning in a serial choice visual discrimination task in the operant 9-hole box by intact and striatal lesioned mice , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  Kelly A. Malcolmson,et al.  The role of the striatum in implicit learning: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2005, Neuroreport.

[21]  D. Willingham,et al.  Motor Skills Have Diverse Neural Bases: Spared and Impaired Skill Acquisition in Huntington's Disease , 1996 .

[22]  Sarah A. J. Reading,et al.  Functional MRI study of a serial reaction time task in Huntington's disease , 2004, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[23]  J. Vonsattel,et al.  Morphometric Demonstration of Atrophic Changes in the Cerebral Cortex, White Matter, and Neostriatum in Huntington's Disease , 1988, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[24]  J. Brandt,et al.  Long-term retention of implicit learning in Huntington's disease , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  J. Ashe,et al.  Neural correlates of encoding and expression in implicit sequence learning , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  E. Irle,et al.  The differential role of premotor frontal cortex and basal ganglia in motor sequence learning: evidence from focal basal ganglia lesions. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[27]  M. Cudkowicz,et al.  Degeneration of pyramidal projection neurons in Huntington's disease cortex , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[28]  Manish S. Shah,et al.  A novel gene containing a trinucleotide repeat that is expanded and unstable on Huntington's disease chromosomes , 1993, Cell.

[29]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Abstract and Effector-Specific Representations of Motor Sequences Identified with PET , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  J. Doyon,et al.  Role of the striatum, cerebellum and frontal lobes in the automatization of a repeated visuomotor sequence of movements , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  Joseph B. Martin,et al.  Somatostatin and neuropeptide Y concentrations in pathologically graded cases of huntington's disease , 1988, Annals of neurology.

[32]  J. Brandt,et al.  Aspects of procedural memory are differentially impaired in Huntington's disease. , 1990, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[33]  S. Dunnett,et al.  The operant serial implicit learning task reveals early onset motor learning deficits in the HdhQ92 knock‐in mouse model of Huntington's disease , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[34]  R. P. Maguire,et al.  Cerebral activation related to implicit sequence learning in a Double Serial Reaction Time task , 2006, Brain Research.

[35]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Neural Substrates of Response-based Sequence Learning using fMRI , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  Eiichi Naito,et al.  Role of the cerebellum in implicit motor skill learning: a PET study , 2004, Brain Research Bulletin.

[37]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  The striatum and probabilistic implicit sequence learning , 2007, Brain Research.

[38]  G. Pearlson,et al.  Frontal lobe volume in patients with Huntington's disease , 1998, Neurology.

[39]  S Channon,et al.  Dissociation between intentional and incidental sequence learning in Huntington's disease. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  Christopher A. Ross,et al.  Neuronal loss in layers V and VI of cerebral cortex in Huntington's disease , 1991, Neuroscience Letters.

[41]  Jacqueline K. White,et al.  Length-dependent gametic CAG repeat instability in the Huntington's disease knock-in mouse. , 1999, Human molecular genetics.