The Problem of Rapid Variable Creation

Both Marcus (2001) and Jackendoff (2002) have emphasized the importance of finding credible explanations for the occurrence of variables within cognitive representations. Marcus, in particular, has argued that a prevailing form of connectionist modeling, eliminative connectionism, cannot adequately explain crucial forms of human generalization. Eliminative connectionism eschews the use of explicitly represented variables, and the latter, Marcus contends, play an essential role in the forms of generalization that he considers. Recently, van der Velde and de Kamps (2006) proposed a neural blackboard architecture, which they assert to have satisfied the variable representation needs that Marcus and Jackendoff identified. However, this letter argues that closely related variants of Marcus's generalization examples possess variable requirements that are incompatible with the van der Velde and de Kamps approach. Moreover, it is argued here that these newly proposed variants present a severe challenge not only for eliminative connectionism but for all network training methods that require iterative tuning of synaptic strengths. The letter focuses on generalization cases that necessitate either virtually instantaneous creation of variables or very rapid deployment of preexisting variables within highly novel contexts.

[1]  John A. Barnden,et al.  Encoding techniques for complex information structures in connectionist systems , 1991 .

[2]  Lokendra Shastri,et al.  Comparing the neural blackboard and the temporal synchrony-based SHRUTI architectures , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[3]  Robert F. Hadley A defence of functional modularity , 2003, Connect. Sci..

[4]  Tony A. Plate,et al.  Analogy retrieval and processing with distributed vector representations , 2000, Expert Syst. J. Knowl. Eng..

[5]  Dirk V. Arnold,et al.  Syntactic systematicity arising from semantic predictions in a Hebbian-competitive network , 2001, Connect. Sci..

[6]  Z. Harris,et al.  Foundations of language , 1941 .

[7]  Robert F. Hadley Systematicity in Connectionist Language Learning , 1994 .

[8]  Robert F. Hadley Connectionism and Novel Combinations of Skills: Implications for Cognitive Architecture , 1999, Minds and Machines.

[9]  Robert F. Hadley Connectionism, explicit rules, and symbolic manipulation , 1993, Minds and Machines.

[10]  Robert F. Hadley,et al.  Generalization in Simple Recurrent Netrworks , 2001 .

[11]  Robert F. Hadley Neural circuits, matrices, and conjunctive binding , 2006 .

[12]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.

[13]  J. Elman Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small , 1993, Cognition.

[14]  R. Jackendoff Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution , 2002 .

[15]  L. Shastri,et al.  From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables and dynamic bindings using temporal synchrony , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[16]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Finding Structure in Time , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[17]  Dmitri A. Rachkovskij,et al.  Binding and Normalization of Binary Sparse Distributed Representations by Context-Dependent Thinning , 2001, Neural Computation.

[18]  B. Connors,et al.  Short-term synaptic enhancement and long-term potentiation in neocortex. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  David Kirsh,et al.  When is Information Explicitly Represented , 1990 .

[20]  Robert F. Hadley,et al.  Strong Semantic Systematicity from Hebbian Connectionist Learning , 1997, Minds and Machines.

[21]  G. Marcus Rethinking Eliminative Connectionism , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Distributed representations and nested compositional structure , 1994 .

[23]  P. Kanerva Fully Distributed Representation , 1997 .

[24]  W. Regehr,et al.  Short-term synaptic plasticity. , 2002, Annual review of physiology.

[25]  Robert F. Hadley,et al.  Two Apparent ‘Counterexamples’ To Marcus: A Closer Look , 2005, Minds and Machines.

[26]  Robert F. Hadley A Defense of Functional Modularity , 2022 .

[27]  Jane Neumann,et al.  Learning the systematic transformation of holographic reduced representations , 2002, Cognitive Systems Research.

[28]  Geoffrey E. Hinton Tensor Product Variable Binding and the Representation of Symbolic Structures in Connectionist Systems , 1991 .

[29]  Robert F. Hadley,et al.  Language acquisition from sparse input without error feedback , 1999, Neural Networks.

[30]  Robert F. Hadley Synchronous versus conjunctive binding: a false dichotomy? , 2007, Connect. Sci..

[31]  Ann Grbavec Second-order generalization in neural networks , 2005 .

[32]  G. Marcus The Algebraic Mind: Integrating Connectionism and Cognitive Science , 2001 .

[33]  Robert F. Hadley The “explicit-implicit” distinction , 1995, Minds and Machines.

[34]  F. van der Velde,et al.  Neural blackboard architectures of combinatorial structures in cognition , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[35]  Ross W. Gayler,et al.  Multiplicative Binding, Representation Operators & Analogy , 1998 .