Stable individual differences in search strategy? The effect of task demands and motivational factors on scanning strategy in visual search.

Previous studies have demonstrated large individual differences in scanning strategy during a dynamic visual search task (E. Becic, A. F. Kramer, & W. R. Boot, 2007; W. R. Boot, A. F. Kramer, E. Becic, D. A. Wiegmann, & T. Kubose, 2006). These differences accounted for substantial variance in performance. Participants who chose to search covertly (without eye movements) excelled, participants who searched overtly (with eye movements) performed poorly. The aim of the current study was to investigate the stability of scanning strategies across different visual search tasks in an attempt to explain why a large percentage of observers might engage in maladaptive strategies. Scanning strategy was assessed for a group of observers across a variety of search tasks without feedback (efficient search, inefficient search, change detection, dynamic search). While scanning strategy was partly determined by task demands, stable individual differences emerged. Participants who searched either overtly or covertly tended to adopt the same strategy regardless of the demands of the search task, even in tasks in which such a strategy was maladaptive. However, when participants were given explicit feedback about their performance during search and performance incentives, strategies across tasks diverged. Thus it appears that observers by default will favor a particular search strategy but can modify this strategy when it is clearly maladaptive to the task.

[1]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  M. Stella Atkins,et al.  Eye gaze patterns differentiate novice and experts in a virtual laparoscopic surgery training environment , 2004, ETRA.

[3]  James T. Enns,et al.  Relax! Cognitive strategy influences visual search , 2006 .

[4]  H. Egeth,et al.  Attention on autopilot: Past experience and attentional set , 2006 .

[5]  J. Henderson,et al.  The Role of Fixation Position in Detecting Scene Changes Across Saccades , 1999 .

[6]  John M Henderson,et al.  Stable individual differences across images in human saccadic eye movements. , 2008, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[7]  W. Boot,et al.  Age-related differences in visual search in dynamic displays. , 2007, Psychology and aging.

[8]  A. Williams,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Journal of Sports Sciences Visual Search, Anticipation and Expertise in Soccer Goalkeepers Visual Search, Anticipation and Expertise in Soccer Goalkeepers , 2022 .

[9]  Carrick C. Williams,et al.  Eye movements during information processing tasks: Individual differences and cultural effects , 2007, Vision Research.

[10]  James R. Brockmole,et al.  Using real-world scenes as contextual cues for search , 2006 .

[11]  Walter R. Boot,et al.  Detecting Transient Changes in Dynamic Displays: The More You Look, the Less You See , 2006, Hum. Factors.

[12]  K. Ball,et al.  Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. , 1993, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[13]  R T Zacks,et al.  Suppression of reflexive saccades in younger and older adults: Age comparisons on an antisaccade task , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[14]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[15]  C. Wickens,et al.  Visual scanning and pilot expertise: the role of attentional flexibility and mental model development. , 1997, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[16]  C. Koch,et al.  A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[17]  Michael S. Ambinder,et al.  Change blindness , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  J. Theeuwes Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  R W Remington,et al.  The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  D. Coppola,et al.  Idiosyncratic characteristics of saccadic eye movements when viewing different visual environments , 1999, Vision Research.

[24]  G. Zelinsky,et al.  Eye movements during parallel-serial visual search. , 1997 .

[25]  Shawn E Christ,et al.  Abrupt onsets cannot be ignored , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  It’s under control: Top-down search strategies can override attentional capture , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[27]  L. Itti Quantitative modelling of perceptual salience at human eye position , 2006 .

[28]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Conversation Disrupts Change Detection in Complex Traffic Scenes , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[29]  David L. Sheinberg,et al.  Eye movements during parallel-serial visual search. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Eye movements during visual search: the costs of choosing the optimal path , 2001, Vision Research.

[31]  J. McCarley,et al.  Automatic and intentional memory processes in visual search , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  D. Roenker,et al.  Age and visual search: expanding the useful field of view. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[33]  Michael L. Mack,et al.  VISUAL SALIENCY DOES NOT ACCOUNT FOR EYE MOVEMENTS DURING VISUAL SEARCH IN REAL-WORLD SCENES , 2007 .

[34]  M. Rendell,et al.  Skill and role-specific differences in eye movement behaviour between goal keepers and field players in field hockey‥ , 2005 .

[35]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  Contextual cueing reduces interference from task-irrelevant onset distractors , 2001 .

[36]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  C. S. Green,et al.  Action video game modifies visual selective attention , 2003, Nature.

[38]  Dietmar Heinke,et al.  Top-down guidance of visual search: A computational account , 2006 .

[39]  J. Henderson,et al.  Inhibitory processes in adults with persistent childhood onset ADHD. , 2002, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[40]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Attentional control during visual search: the effect of irrelevant singletons. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.