Heritability of Working Memory Brain Activation

Although key to understanding individual variation in task-related brain activation, the genetic contribution to these individual differences remains largely unknown. Here we report voxel-by-voxel genetic model fitting in a large sample of 319 healthy, young adult, human identical and fraternal twins (mean ± SD age, 23.6 ± 1.8 years) who performed an n-back working memory task during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) at a high magnetic field (4 tesla). Patterns of task-related brain response (BOLD signal difference of 2-back minus 0-back) were significantly heritable, with the highest estimates (40–65%) in the inferior, middle, and superior frontal gyri, left supplementary motor area, precentral and postcentral gyri, middle cingulate cortex, superior medial gyrus, angular gyrus, superior parietal lobule, including precuneus, and superior occipital gyri. Furthermore, high test-retest reliability for a subsample of 40 twins indicates that nongenetic variance in the fMRI brain response is largely due to unique environmental influences rather than measurement error. Individual variations in activation of the working memory network are therefore significantly influenced by genetic factors. By establishing the heritability of cognitive brain function in a large sample that affords good statistical power, and using voxel-by-voxel analyses, this study provides the necessary evidence for task-related brain activation to be considered as an endophenotype for psychiatric or neurological disorders, and represents a substantial new contribution to the field of neuroimaging genetics. These genetic brain maps should facilitate discovery of gene variants influencing cognitive brain function through genome-wide association studies, potentially opening up new avenues in the treatment of brain disorders.

[1]  H. Grüneberg,et al.  Introduction to quantitative genetics , 1960 .

[2]  J. Fleiss,et al.  Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  T. H. Harrell,et al.  Computerized versus traditional administration of the multidimensional aptitude battery-verbal scale: An examination of reliability and validity , 1987 .

[4]  A. Thapar,et al.  Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families , 1993 .

[5]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[6]  R. Coppola,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Brain Mapping in Psychiatry: Methodological Issues Illustrated in a Study of Working Memory in Schizophrenia , 1998, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[7]  J. Ashburner,et al.  Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[8]  A. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis II: Inflation, Flattening, and a Surface-Based Coordinate System , 1999, NeuroImage.

[9]  Y. Ono,et al.  Genetic Structure of Spatial and Verbal Working Memory , 2001, Behavior genetics.

[10]  G. Geffen,et al.  Genetic Covariance Among Measures of Information Processing Speed, Working Memory, and IQ , 2001, Behavior genetics.

[11]  R. Henson,et al.  Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  Hans Forssberg,et al.  Increased Brain Activity in Frontal and Parietal Cortex Underlies the Development of Visuospatial Working Memory Capacity during Childhood , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  R Todd Constable,et al.  Image distortion correction in EPI: Comparison of field mapping with point spread function mapping , 2002, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[14]  Jean-Francois Mangin,et al.  What is the best similarity measure for motion correction in fMRI time series? , 2002, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[15]  Margaret J. Wright,et al.  A genetic two-factor model of the covariation among a subset of Multidimensional Aptitude Battery and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised subtests , 2003 .

[16]  M Zaitsev,et al.  Error reduction and parameter optimization of the TAPIR method for fast T1 mapping , 2003, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[17]  M. Egan,et al.  Abnormal fMRI response of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in cognitively intact siblings of patients with schizophrenia. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.

[18]  Suzanne E. Welcome,et al.  Mapping cortical change across the human life span , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  I. Gottesman,et al.  The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.

[20]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Intelligence: genetics, genes, and genomics. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  Margaret J. Wright,et al.  Brisbane Adolescent Twin Study: Outline of study methods and research projects , 2004 .

[22]  L. Fazekas,et al.  The Coronary Effects of Parathyroid Hormone , 2004, Hormone Research in Paediatrics.

[23]  Rajesh Kumar,et al.  A method for removal of global effects from fMRI time series , 2004, NeuroImage.

[24]  A. Saykin,et al.  Brain activation patterns associated with working memory in relapsing-remitting MS , 2004, Neurology.

[25]  R. Gibbs,et al.  Variation in GRM3 affects cognition, prefrontal glutamate, and risk for schizophrenia. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  Peter M Visscher,et al.  Power of the classical twin design revisited. , 2004, Twin research : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies.

[27]  Susan F Tapert,et al.  fMRI reveals alteration of spatial working memory networks across adolescence , 2005, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[28]  Kathryn M. McMillan,et al.  N‐back working memory paradigm: A meta‐analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[29]  Stefan Krumm,et al.  Reasoning=working memory¿attention , 2005 .

[30]  Danielle Posthuma,et al.  A longitudinal twin study on IQ, executive functioning, and attention problems during childhood and early adolescence. , 2006, Acta neurologica Belgica.

[31]  Alan H. Wilman,et al.  Males and females differ in brain activation during cognitive tasks , 2006, NeuroImage.

[32]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Correlates of Individual Differences in Memory , 2006, Neuron.

[33]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Nature versus Nurture in Ventral Visual Cortex: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Twins , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Boualem Mensour,et al.  Individual variation in neural correlates of sadness in children: A twin fMRI study , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[35]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic areas revisited , 2007, NeuroImage.

[36]  J. Giedd,et al.  Review of Twin and Family Studies on Neuroanatomic Phenotypes and Typical Neurodevelopment , 2007, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[37]  J L Lancaster,et al.  Prefrontal hyperactivation during working memory task in untreated individuals with major depressive disorder , 2007, Molecular Psychiatry.

[38]  Murray B. Stein,et al.  Heritability of anterior cingulate response to conflict: An fMRI study in female twins , 2007, NeuroImage.

[39]  Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Epistasis between catechol-O-methyltransferase and type II metabotropic glutamate receptor 3 genes on working memory brain function , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[40]  C. Montag,et al.  A functional variant of the tryptophan hydroxylase 2 gene impacts working memory: A genetic imaging study , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[41]  Paul M. Thompson,et al.  Quantifying the heritability of task-related brain activation and performance during the N-back working memory task: A twin fMRI study , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[42]  R. Murray,et al.  Genetic Liability for Bipolar Disorder Is Characterized by Excess Frontal Activation in Response to a Working Memory Task , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[43]  Jay N Giedd,et al.  The changing impact of genes and environment on brain development during childhood and adolescence: Initial findings from a neuroimaging study of pediatric twins , 2008, Development and Psychopathology.

[44]  Steven C. R. Williams,et al.  Measuring fMRI reliability with the intra-class correlation coefficient , 2009, NeuroImage.

[45]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Genetic Contribution to Variation in Cognitive Function: An fMRI Study in Twins , 2009, Science.

[46]  Heike Schmidt,et al.  No gender differences in brain activation during the N‐back task: An fMRI study in healthy individuals , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[47]  Laurie E. Cutting,et al.  Developmental sex differences in basic visuospatial processing: Differences in strategy use? , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[48]  Bart Rypma,et al.  Neural and vascular variability and the fMRI-BOLD response in normal aging. , 2010, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[49]  P. Fox,et al.  Genetic control over the resting brain , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  Ting Li,et al.  Gender-specific hemodynamics in prefrontal cortex during a verbal working memory task by near-infrared spectroscopy , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.