Task-switching in oculomotor control: Unidirectional switch-cost when alternating between pro- and antisaccades

The antisaccade task requires the suppression of a reflexive prosaccade (i.e., response suppression) and the remapping of a target location to mirror-symmetrical space (i.e., vector inversion). Moreover, antisaccades are associated with increased activation of cortical oculomotor networks: a finding attributed to the top-down requirements of response suppression and vector inversion. The goal of the present study was to determine if the increased cortical activity associated with antisaccades elicits a residual inhibition of oculomotor planning networks. To that end, each trial in this investigation entailed the onset of a single and exogenously presented target (i.e., archetypical antisaccade task) and participants were instructed to alternate between pro- and antisaccades in blocked and random task-switching schedules. In the blocked schedule, the saccade tasks (i.e., pro- and antisaccades) alternated on every second trial (AABB paradigm) whereas in the random schedule the saccade tasks were pseudo-randomly interleaved on a trial-by-trial basis. Reaction times for task-switch prosaccades were longer and more variable than their task-repetition counterparts, whereas antisaccades did not vary as a function of task-switch and task-repetition trials: a finding that was consistent across blocked and random presentation schedules. In other words, results demonstrate a unidirectional switch-cost for prosaccades. As such, we propose that the top-down processes required to complete an antisaccade results in residual inhibition of oculomotor networks supporting a subsequent prosaccade.

[1]  Mark W Greenlee,et al.  Neural correlates of inter- and intra-individual saccadic reaction time differences in the gap/overlap paradigm. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Ritske de Jong,et al.  Strategical determinants of compatibility effects with task uncertainty , 1995 .

[3]  D P Munoz,et al.  Role of Primate Superior Colliculus in Preparation and Execution of Anti-Saccades and Pro-Saccades , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .

[5]  Mariya V. Cherkasova,et al.  Antisaccades and task switching: studies of control processes in saccadic function in normal subjects and schizophrenic patients. , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Burkhart Fischer,et al.  On the production and correction of involuntary prosaccades in a gap antisaccade task , 2000, Vision Research.

[7]  S Monsell,et al.  Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? , 2000, Psychological research.

[8]  N. Yeung,et al.  Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Matthew R. G. Brown,et al.  Neural processes associated with antisaccade task performance investigated with event-related FMRI. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  O. Tzeng,et al.  Trial type probability modulates the cost of antisaccades. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  D. Munoz,et al.  Reflex suppression in the anti-saccade task is dependent on prestimulus neural processes. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Dara S. Manoach,et al.  Antisaccades and task-switching: interactions in controlled processing , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Jun Zhang,et al.  Flexible filaments in a flowing soap film as a model for one-dimensional flags in a two-dimensional wind , 2000, Nature.

[14]  Matthew S. Cain,et al.  Neural Activity Is Modulated by Trial History: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of the Effects of a Previous Antisaccade , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  The role of inhibition in task switching: A review , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[16]  Dan Milea,et al.  Cortical Mechanisms of Saccade Generation from Execution to Decision , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[17]  Dara S. Manoach,et al.  Task-switching with antisaccades versus no-go trials: a comparison of inter-trial effects , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  D P Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Correlates for Preparatory Set Associated with Pro-Saccades and Anti-Saccades in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Mariya V. Cherkasova,et al.  Antisaccades and Task Switching , 2002 .

[20]  J. Barton,et al.  Task-switching in schizophrenia: active switching costs and passive carry-over effects in an antisaccade paradigm , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  M. Heath,et al.  The prior-antisaccade effect influences the planning and online control of prosaccades , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  A comparison of frontoparietal fMRI activation during anti-saccades and anti-pointing. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[24]  C. Curtis,et al.  Success and Failure Suppressing Reflexive Behavior , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  A. Allport,et al.  Bilingual Language Switching in Naming: Asymmetrical Costs of Language Selection , 1999 .

[26]  A. Riehle,et al.  Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Why are antisaccades slower than prosaccades? A novel finding using a new paradigm , 2003, Neuroreport.

[28]  Mariya V. Cherkasova,et al.  Schizophrenic subjects show deficient inhibition but intact task switching on saccadic tasks , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[29]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[30]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[31]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Electroencephalographic evidence of vector inversion in antipointing , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  D. Rosenbaum Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[33]  Dara S. Manoach,et al.  Switching, plasticity, and prediction in a saccadic task-switch paradigm , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[35]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Antisaccade performance predicted by neuronal activity in the supplementary eye field , 1997, Nature.

[36]  S. Everling,et al.  The antisaccade: a review of basic research and clinical studies , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  Burkhart Fischer,et al.  Effects of procues on error rate and reaction times of antisaccades in human subjects , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  D. Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Activity in Monkey Superior Colliculus Related to the Initiation of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  G. Schöner,et al.  Preshaping and continuous evolution of motor cortical representations during movement preparation , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[40]  M. Heath,et al.  Antisaccades exhibit diminished online control relative to prosaccades , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in humans investigated with event-related FMRI. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[44]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  Neuronal switching of sensorimotor transformations for antisaccades , 2000, Nature.

[45]  Stefan Everling,et al.  Frontoparietal activation with preparation for antisaccades. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.