Contrasting Cortical and Subcortical Activations Produced by Attentional-Set Shifting and Reversal Learning in Humans

Much evidence suggests that lesions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) produce marked impairments in the ability of subjects to shift cognitive set, as exemplified by performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). However, studies with humans and experimental primates have suggested that damage to different regions of PFC induce dissociable impairments in two forms of shift learning implicit in the WCST (that is, extradimensional (ED) shift learning and reversal shift learning), with similar deficits also being apparent after damage to basal ganglia structures, especially the caudate nucleus. In this study, we used the same visual discrimination learning paradigm over multidimensional stimuli, and the H215O positron emission tomography (PET) technique, to examine regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) changes associated with these subcomponent processes of the WCST. In three conditions, subjects were scanned while acquiring visual discriminations involving either (i) the same stimulus dimension as preceding discriminations (intradimensional (ID) shifts); (ii) different stimulus dimensions from previous discriminations (ED shifts) or (iii) reversed stimulus-reward contingencies (reversal shifts). Additionally, subjects were scanned while responding to already learnt discriminations (performance baseline). ED shift learning, relative to ID shift learning, produced activations in prefrontal regions, including, left anterior PFC and right dorsolateral PFC (BA 10 and 9/46). By contrast, reversal learning, relative to ID shift learning, produced activations of the left caudate nucleus. Additionally, compared to reversal and ID shift learning, ED shift learning was associated with relative deactivations in occipito-temporal pathways (for example, BA 17 and 37). These results confirm that, in the context of visual discrimination learning over multidimensional stimuli, the control of an acquired attentional bias or 'set', and the control of previously acquired stimulus-reinforcement associations, activate distinct cortical and subcortical neural stations. Moreover, we propose that the PFC may contribute to the control of attentional-set by modulating attentional processes mediated by occipito-temporal pathways.

[1]  D. A. Grant,et al.  A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. , 1948, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  M. B. Bender,et al.  Performance of complex visual tasks after cerebral lesions. , 1951, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[3]  I. David Isaacs,et al.  Reversal and nonreversal shifts within and between dimensions in concept formation. , 1962, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  B. Milner Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .

[5]  B. Shepp,et al.  INTRADIMENSIONAL AND EXTRADIMENSIONAL SHIFTS IN THE RAT. , 1964, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[6]  E. Lovejoy An attention theory of discrimination learning , 1965 .

[7]  N. Mackintosh SELECTIVE ATTENTION IN ANIMAL DISCRIMINATION LEARNING. , 1965, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Behavioral effects of selective ablation of the caudate nucleus. , 1967, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[9]  N. J. Slamecka A methodological analysis of shift paradigms in human discrimination learning. , 1968, Psychological bulletin.

[10]  M Mishkin,et al.  Further evidence on the locus of the visual area in the temporal lobe of the monkey. , 1969, Experimental neurology.

[11]  A. M. Schrier,et al.  Consecutive intradimensional and extradimensional shifts in monkeys. , 1969 .

[12]  T. Powell,et al.  An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways within the cerebral cortex of the monkey. , 1970, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning , 1971 .

[14]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Limbic lesions and the problem of stimulus--reinforcement associations. , 1972, Experimental neurology.

[15]  David Zeaman,et al.  An Attention-Retention Theory of Retardate Discrimination Learning1 , 1973 .

[16]  Judy Bond,et al.  Regional Cerebral Blood Flow , 1974, Journal of neurosurgical nursing.

[17]  E. Drewe,et al.  The effect of type and area of brain lesion on Wisconsin card sorting test performance. , 1974, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[18]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  M. M. Burns,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1975, Neurology.

[20]  H. Nelson A Modified Card Sorting Test Sensitive to Frontal Lobe Defects , 1976, Cortex.

[21]  Beth C Stoneking,et al.  Test-Retest Evaluations of Attention and Rigidity , 1978 .

[22]  A. Murray Harper,et al.  Regional Cerebral Blood Flow , 1979 .

[23]  S. Zola-Morgan,et al.  Comparative neuropsychology and Korsakoff's syndrome. I—Spatial and visual reversal learning , 1980, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  R. Heaton,et al.  The utility of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in detecting and localizing frontal lobe lesions. , 1980, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[25]  S. Zola-Morgan,et al.  Comparative neuropsychology and Korsakoff's syndrome. II—Two-choice visual discrimination learning , 1980, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  D. Robinson,et al.  Behavioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. I. Modulation in posterior parietal cortex related to selective visual attention. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[28]  E. Mancall,et al.  Development of neuropsychological deficits in Huntington's disease. , 1983, Archives of neurology.

[29]  M. Albert,et al.  Varieties of perseveration , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Functional mapping of the human cerebellum with positron emission tomography. , 1985, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation , 1985, Neurology.

[32]  J. Bryer,et al.  Superior sorting and categorizing ability in a case of bilateral frontal atrophy: an exception to the rule. , 1986, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[33]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[34]  D. Weinberger,et al.  Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. II. Role of neuroleptic treatment, attention, and mental effort. , 1986, Archives of general psychiatry.

[35]  D. Weinberger,et al.  Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. I. Regional cerebral blood flow evidence. , 1986, Archives of general psychiatry.

[36]  M. Albert,et al.  Perseveration in behavioral neurology , 1987, Neurology.

[37]  H. Sakai,et al.  Enhancement of inferior temporal neurons during visual discrimination. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  T. Robbins,et al.  The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[39]  A. Wyler,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with complex partial seizures of temporal-lobe origin. , 1988, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[40]  A. C. Roberts,et al.  Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a specific attentional dysfunction , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  B Jonas,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance Based on Location and Size of Neuroanatomical Lesion in Vietnam Veterans with Penetrating Head Injury , 1990, Perceptual and motor skills.

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Relationship between Global and Local Changes in PET Scans , 1990, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[43]  H. Damasio,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance as a measure of frontal lobe damage. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[44]  T. Robbins,et al.  Extra-dimensional versus intra-dimensional set shifting performance following frontal lobe excisions, temporal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  Shelley Channon,et al.  T-Maze Discrimination and Reversal Learning After Unilateral Temporal or Frontal Lobe Lesions in Man , 1991, Cortex.

[46]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow during voluntary arm and hand movements in human subjects. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[48]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[49]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[50]  K. Boone,et al.  Major depressives' and dysthmics' performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. , 1991, Journal of clinical psychology.

[51]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  An MRI-based stereotactic atlas from 250 young normal subjects , 1992 .

[52]  T. Robbins,et al.  A specific form of cognitive rigidity following excitotoxic lesions of the basal forebrain in marmosets , 1992, Neuroscience.

[53]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Subcortical connections of inferior temporal areas TE and TEO in macaque monkeys , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[54]  C. Bradshaw,et al.  Utility of the Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in neuropsychological assessment. , 1993, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[55]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[56]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[57]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[58]  John Duncan,et al.  A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex , 1993, Nature.

[59]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory , 1993, Nature.

[60]  E. Rolls,et al.  Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[61]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Cortical control of saccades and fixation in man. A PET study. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[62]  M. Corbetta,et al.  PET studies of parietal involvement in spatial attention: comparison of different task types. , 1994, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[63]  A C Roberts,et al.  6-Hydroxydopamine lesions of the prefrontal cortex in monkeys enhance performance on an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: possible interactions with subcortical dopamine , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[64]  A. Thierry,et al.  Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Prefrontal Cortex , 1994, Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences.

[65]  P. Fox,et al.  Computational approaches to network analysis in functional brain imaging , 1994 .

[66]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  The Issue of Memory in the Study of Prefrontal Function , 1994 .

[67]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[68]  B. Motter Neural correlates of attentive selection for color or luminance in extrastriate area V4 , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[69]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Distinct neural correlates of visual long-term memory for spatial location and object identity: a positron emission tomography study in humans. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[70]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Functional Brain Imaging Studies of Cortical Mechanisms for Memory , 1995, Science.

[71]  D. Pandya,et al.  Corticostriatal connections of extrastriate visual areas in rhesus monkeys. , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[72]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[73]  M. Petrides,et al.  Functional Organization of the Human Frontal Cortex for Mnemonic Processing. , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[74]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Superior Parietal Cortex Activation During Spatial Attention Shifts and Visual Feature Conjunction , 1995, Science.

[75]  S. Folstein,et al.  Early Loss of Neostriatal Striosome Neurons in Huntington's Disease , 1995, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[76]  G. Winocur,et al.  Dissociation of pathways for object and spatial vision: a PET study in humans , 1995, Neuroreport.

[77]  P. Goldman-Rakic Architecture of the Prefrontal Cortex and the Central Executive , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[78]  C. Marsden,et al.  Comparison of executive and visuospatial memory function in Huntington's disease and dementia of Alzheimer type matched for degree of dementia. , 1995, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[79]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  The neurology of visual attention. , 1995 .

[80]  J H Maunsell,et al.  The Brain's Visual World: Representation of Visual Targets in Cerebral Cortex , 1995, Science.

[81]  Richard Coppola,et al.  Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A positron emission tomography study , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[82]  E. Lynd-Balta,et al.  The orbital and medial prefrontal circuit through the primate basal ganglia , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[83]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Activation of human prefrontal cortex during spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks measured by functional MRI. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[84]  John K. Kruschke,et al.  Dimensional Relevance Shifts in Category Learning , 1996, Connect. Sci..

[85]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Isolating the Mnemonic Component in Spatial Delayed Response: A Controlled PET15O-Labeled Water Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Study in Normal Humans , 1996, NeuroImage.

[86]  E. Rolls,et al.  The Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2019 .

[87]  A C Roberts,et al.  Primate analogue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: effects of excitotoxic lesions of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[88]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[89]  J. Jonides,et al.  Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[90]  T. Robbins,et al.  Executive and mnemonic functions in early Huntington's disease. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[91]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.

[92]  D. Gaffan Memory, action and the corpus striatum: current developments in the memory-habit distinction , 1996 .

[93]  P. Strick,et al.  The temporal lobe is a target of output from the basal ganglia. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[94]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts , 1996, Nature.

[95]  L. Goldstein The frontal lobes and voluntary action , 1996 .

[96]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable Forms of Inhibitory Control within Prefrontal Cortex with an Analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: Restriction to Novel Situations and Independence from “On-Line” Processing , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[97]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Incorporating Prior Knowledge into Image Registration , 1997, NeuroImage.

[98]  A. Owen The Functional Organization of Working Memory Processes Within Human Lateral Frontal Cortex: The Contribution of Functional Neuroimaging , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[99]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[100]  D. Pandya,et al.  Corticostriatal connections of extrastriate visual areas in rhesus monkeys , 1997, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[101]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[102]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Transient activation of inferior prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[103]  R. K. Simpson Nature Neuroscience , 2022 .