Bias resulting from the use of ‘assay sensitivity’ as an inclusion criterion for meta‐analysis

Assay sensitivity has been proposed as a criterion for including psychiatric clinical outcome studies in meta‐analyses. The authors assess the performance of assay sensitivity as a method for determining study appropriateness for meta‐analysis by calculating expected standard drug vs placebo effect sizes for various combinations of high quality and flawed studies. In the absence of flawed studies, expected effect sizes are close to unbiased only when sample sizes are very large. In the presence of flawed studies, expected effect sizes tend to be substantially biased except under simultaneous conditions of high power, a large proportion of flawed studies, and a population standard vs placebo effect size of flawed studies considerably lower than that of high quality studies. The authors conclude that this method is not robust and can lead to serious bias. Unless it can be shown that specific conditions hold, assay sensitivity should not be used to make quality judgments of studies. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

[1]  D. Moher,et al.  The CONSORT statement: revised recommendations for improving the quality of reports of parallel-group randomized trials. , 2001, Journal of the American Podiatric Medical Association.

[2]  D. Moher,et al.  The Revised CONSORT Statement for Reporting Randomized Trials: Explanation and Elaboration , 2001, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[3]  D. Klein,et al.  Flawed meta-analyses comparing psychotherapy with pharmacotherapy. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[4]  M K Shear,et al.  Cognitive-behavioral therapy, imipramine, or their combination for panic disorder: A randomized controlled trial. , 2000, JAMA.

[5]  Paul Leber,et al.  The use of placebo control groups in the assessment of psychiatric drugs: an historical context , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[6]  H. Kraemer,et al.  Use of placebo control groups in evaluating efficacy of treatment of unipolar major depression , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[7]  D. Klein,et al.  Validity of clinical trials of antidepressants. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[8]  J. Maser,et al.  Cognitive Behavior Therapy and Pharmacotherapy for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: The NIMH-Sponsored Collaborative Study , 1999 .

[9]  M. Egger,et al.  The hazards of scoring the quality of clinical trials for meta-analysis. , 1999, JAMA.

[10]  D. Cook,et al.  Does quality of reports of randomised trials affect estimates of intervention efficacy reported in meta-analyses? , 1998, The Lancet.

[11]  R. Joffe,et al.  Antidepressant Treatment of Depression: A Metaanalysis , 1996, Canadian journal of psychiatry. Revue canadienne de psychiatrie.

[12]  D. Klein Critiquing McNally's reply , 1996 .

[13]  I. Olkin,et al.  Improving the quality of reporting of randomized controlled trials. The CONSORT statement. , 1996, JAMA.

[14]  A R Jadad,et al.  Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: is blinding necessary? , 1996, Controlled clinical trials.

[15]  V. De Gruttola,et al.  Modelling progression of CD4-lymphocyte count and its relationship to survival time. , 1994, Biometrics.

[16]  M. Cho,et al.  Instruments for assessing the quality of drug studies published in the medical literature. , 1994, JAMA.

[17]  S D Imber,et al.  National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program. General effectiveness of treatments. , 1989, Archives of general psychiatry.

[18]  P. Billingsley,et al.  Probability and Measure , 1980 .

[19]  D. Klein Control groups in pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy evaluations. , 1997 .

[20]  M. Pollack,et al.  A meta-analysis of treatment outcome for panic disorder , 1995 .

[21]  D F Klein,et al.  COGNITIVE THERAPY , 2016 .