Hand use for grasping in a bimanual task: evidence for different roles?

It has been proposed that the two hands play different roles during bimanual object interaction. The right hand takes on an explorative, highly precise, manipulative role while the left hand supports and stabilizes the object. Does this division of labour influence hand use during visually guided grasping? Three experiments were designed to address this question: right-handed individuals put together 3D models using big or small building blocks scattered across a tabletop. Participants were free to build the models; however, it felt comfortable (Experiment 1) or they were required to build on a large (Experiment 2) or small (Experiment 3) base plate. In Experiment 1, the right hand was preferred for grasping while the left hand stabilized the building model. When participants used the large base plate (Experiment 2), right hand use for grasping decreased and left hand use increased. The plate provided freedom to the left hand from having to stabilize the building model, but it also interfered with right/left hand movements directed towards the opposite side of the grasping hand (contralateral movements). To investigate which of these two factors would explain the change in hand use for grasping, a very small base plate was used in the last experiment. Results showed similar right hand use values to those seen in the first experiment (without the use of a plate), even though the left hand was ‘released from its stabilizing duties.’ The results predict a left-hemisphere right hand advantage in the control of grasping.

[1]  Paolo Bartolomeo,et al.  A parietofrontal network for spatial awareness in the right hemisphere of the human brain. , 2006, Archives of neurology.

[2]  W. Hopkins,et al.  Hand preferences for a coordinated bimanual task in 110 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): cross-sectional analysis. , 1995, Journal of comparative psychology.

[3]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Bimanual strategies for object retrieval in infants and young children , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  David S. Vogel,et al.  Cerebral lateralization of spatial abilities: A meta-analysis , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[5]  S. Jackson,et al.  Attention for action: coordinating bimanual reach-to-grasp movements. , 1999, British journal of psychology.

[6]  Roland R. Lee,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries for kinematic and positional aspects of reaching. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  Carl Gabbard,et al.  TASK COMPLEXITY AND LIMB SELECTION IN REACHING , 2003, The International journal of neuroscience.

[8]  R. Johansson,et al.  Selection of Prime Actor in Humans during Bimanual Object Manipulation , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  G. Calvert,et al.  Quantifying hand preference using a behavioural continuum. , 1998, Laterality.

[10]  Andrea H. Mason,et al.  Coordination and control of bimanual prehension: effects of perturbing object location , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  U. Castiello,et al.  Cortical Activations in Humans Grasp-Related Areas Depend on Hand Used and Handedness , 2008, PloS one.

[12]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  Higher Brain Functions , 2008 .

[13]  M. Carlier,et al.  Midline crossing: Developmental trend from 3 to 10 years of age in a preferential card-reaching task , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[14]  Tzvi Ganel,et al.  Hemispheric specialization for the visual control of action is independent of handedness. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  M. Bryden,et al.  A behavioral measure of hand preference as opposed to hand skill , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  D Goodman,et al.  On the coordination of two-handed movements. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Robert L. Whitwell,et al.  Left handedness does not extend to visually guided precision grasping , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  L. E. Rohr,et al.  The effects of skill demands and object position on the distribution of preferred hand reaches , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[19]  C. Gabbard,et al.  A lateralized comparison of handedness and object proximity. , 1997, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[20]  D. Carey,et al.  Bimanual reaching across the hemispace: Which hand is yoked to which? , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[21]  M Wiesendanger,et al.  Kinematics of a coordinated goal-directed bimanual task , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[22]  P. Bright,et al.  The measurement of hand preference: a validation study comparing three groups of right-handers. , 1996, British journal of psychology.

[23]  Bill Jones,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries in motor control of guided reaching with and without optic displacement , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  P. Bryden,et al.  Preferential reaching across regions of hemispace in adults and children. , 2006, Developmental psychobiology.

[25]  L. E. Rohr,et al.  Reaching patterns across working space: The effects of handedness, task demands, and comfort levels , 2006, Laterality.

[26]  J. Fagard,et al.  The effect of task constraints on infants’ (bi)manual strategy for grasping and exploring objects , 2005 .

[27]  C. Gabbard,et al.  What Determines Choice of Limb for Unimanual Reaching Movements? , 2000, The Journal of general psychology.

[28]  G. Vallar,et al.  Spatial frames of reference and somatosensory processing: a neuropsychological perspective. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[29]  M. Goodale Hemispheric differences in motor control , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.

[30]  M Wiesendanger,et al.  Time structure of a goal-directed bimanual skill and its dependence on task constraints , 1999, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  L. E. Rohr,et al.  The performance of left-handed participants on a preferential reaching test , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[32]  R. Johansson,et al.  How a Lateralized Brain Supports Symmetrical Bimanual Tasks , 2006, PLoS biology.

[33]  R. Steenhuis The relation between hand preference and hand performance: what you get depends on what you measure. , 1999, Laterality.

[34]  J. Fagard,et al.  Unimanual and bimanual tasks and the assessment of handedness in toddlers , 2000 .

[35]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Y. Guiard Asymmetric division of labor in human skilled bimanual action: the kinematic chain as a model. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.

[37]  Ruud G. J. Meulenbroek,et al.  Behavioral evidence for left-hemisphere specialization of motor planning , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Jenni M. Karl,et al.  Development of rotational movements, hand shaping, and accuracy in advance and withdrawal for the reach-to-eat movement in human infants aged 6-12 months. , 2012, Infant behavior & development.

[39]  J. Annett,et al.  The Control of Movement in the Preferred and Non-Preferred Hands* , 1979, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[40]  Eric A Roy,et al.  Using hand performance measures to predict handedness , 2006, Laterality.

[41]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Dynamics of hemispheric specialization and integration in the context of motor control , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[42]  Coordination et prevalence manuelle chez le nourrisson F. Flament edition du C.N.R.S., Marseille, 1975, 278pp. , 1978, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  The dominant and nondominant arms are specialized for stabilizing different features of task performance , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  M. Goodale,et al.  Hand preference for precision grasping predicts language lateralization , 2009, Neuropsychologia.