Opposed optimal strategies of weighting somatosensory inputs for planning reaching movements toward visual and proprioceptive targets.

Behavioral studies have suggested that the brain uses a visual estimate of the hand to plan reaching movements toward visual targets and somatosensory inputs in the case of somatosensory targets. However, neural correlates for distinct coding of the hand according to the sensory modality of the target have not yet been identified. Here we tested the twofold hypothesis that the somatosensory input from the reaching hand is facilitated and inhibited, respectively, when planning movements toward somatosensory (unseen fingers) or visual targets. The weight of the somatosensory inputs was assessed by measuring the amplitude of the somatosensory evoked potential (SEP) resulting from vibration of the reaching finger during movement planning. The target sensory modality had no significant effect on SEP amplitude. However, Spearman's analyses showed significant correlations between the SEPs and reaching errors. When planning movements toward proprioceptive targets without visual feedback of the reaching hand, participants showing the greater SEPs were those who produced the smaller directional errors. Inversely, participants showing the smaller SEPs when planning movements toward visual targets with visual feedback of the reaching hand were those who produced the smaller directional errors. No significant correlation was found between the SEPs and radial or amplitude errors. Our results indicate that the sensory strategy for planning movements is highly flexible among individuals and also for a given sensory context. Most importantly, they provide neural bases for the suggestion that optimization of movement planning requires the target and the reaching hand to both be represented in the same sensory modality.

[1]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Multiple levels of representation of reaching in the parieto-frontal network. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  M. Latash,et al.  Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Vectorial coding of movement: vision, proprioception, or both? , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  J. Gordon,et al.  Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  Hannah J. Block,et al.  Sensory weighting and realignment: independent compensatory processes. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  Jacques Felblinger,et al.  Automated cortical projection of EEG sensors: Anatomical correlation via the international 10–10 system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[7]  J. Saunders,et al.  Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  J. Rohrbaugh,et al.  Improving spatial and temporal resolution in evoked EEG responses using surface Laplacians. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[9]  Sandra E. Black,et al.  Task-Relevant Modulation of Contralateral and Ipsilateral Primary Somatosensory Cortex and the Role of a Prefrontal-Cortical Sensory Gating System , 2002, NeuroImage.

[10]  David J. Sheskin,et al.  Handbook of Parametric and Nonparametric Statistical Procedures , 1997 .

[11]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Human Posterior Parietal Cortex Flexibly Determines Reference Frames for Reaching Based on Sensory Context , 2010, Neuron.

[12]  Y Rossetti,et al.  Vision for spatial perception and vision for action: a dissociation between the left–right and near–far dimensions , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  C C Wood,et al.  Developmental and aging changes in somatosensory, auditory and visual evoked potentials. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[14]  El-Mehdi Meftah,et al.  A critical speed for gating of tactile detection during voluntary movement , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  P. Nunez,et al.  A theoretical and experimental study of high resolution EEG based on surface Laplacians and cortical imaging. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  P. Haggard,et al.  Vision of the body modulates somatosensory intracortical inhibition. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  M Desmurget,et al.  Role of the medial part of the intraparietal sulcus in implementing movement direction. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Analysis of Pointing Errors Reveals Properties of Data Representations and Coordinate Transformations Within the Central Nervous System , 2000, Neural Computation.

[19]  R. J. Seitz,et al.  Visual cortex activation in kinesthetic guidance of reaching , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  The role of execution noise in movement variability. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  K. Yamashiro,et al.  Skill-specific changes in somatosensory-evoked potentials and reaction times in baseball players , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Michele Tagliabue,et al.  Necessity is the Mother of Invention: Reconstructing Missing Sensory Information in Multiple, Concurrent Reference Frames for Eye–Hand Coordination , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Utilization of Visual Feedback Information during Rapid Pointing Movements , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[24]  J F Soechting,et al.  Organization of sequential typing movements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  C. Bard,et al.  Reference systems for coding spatial information in normal subjects and a deafferented patient , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  C. Tenke,et al.  Generator localization by current source density (CSD): Implications of volume conduction and field closure at intracranial and scalp resolutions , 2012, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[27]  W. Staines,et al.  Predictability of the target stimulus for sensory-guided movement modulates early somatosensory cortical potentials , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[28]  J. Rothwell,et al.  Gating of somatosensory evoked potentials during different kinds of movement in man. , 1981, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Differential influence of vision and proprioception on control of movement distance , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Optic ataxia is not only ‘optic’: Impaired spatial integration of proprioceptive information , 2007, NeuroImage.

[31]  J. P. Kalisvaart,et al.  Motion discrimination under uncertainty and ambiguity. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[32]  Michele Tagliabue,et al.  When Kinesthesia Becomes Visual: A Theoretical Justification for Executing Motor Tasks in Visual Space , 2013, PloS one.

[33]  C. Bard,et al.  Control of Rapid Arm Movements When Target Position Is Altered During Saccadic Suppression. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  J. Diedrichsen,et al.  A Dedicated Binding Mechanism for the Visual Control of Movement , 2014, Current Biology.

[35]  Fabrice R Sarlegna,et al.  Visual guidance of arm reaching: online adjustments of movement direction are impaired by amplitude control. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[36]  Febo Cincotti,et al.  Spatial enhancement of EEG data by surface Laplacian estimation: the use of magnetic resonance imaging-based head models , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[37]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  Vision Modulates Somatosensory Cortical Processing , 2002, Current Biology.

[38]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Representing spatial information for limb movement: role of area 5 in the monkey. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  J. Vercher,et al.  Target and hand position information in the online control of goal-directed arm movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  C. Ghez,et al.  Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  A computational perspective on the neural basis of multisensory spatial representations , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[42]  Borís Burle,et al.  Spatio-temporal dynamics of reach-related neural activity for visual and somatosensory targets , 2009, NeuroImage.

[43]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Sensory transformations and the use of multiple reference frames for reach planning , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  T. Vilis,et al.  Integration of target and effector information in human posterior parietal cortex for the planning of action. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  Hannah J. Block,et al.  Sensory reweighting in targeted reaching: effects of conscious effort, error history, and target salience. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Covariation of primate dorsal premotor cell activity with direction and amplitude during a memorized-delay reaching task. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[48]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Distortions in recall from visual memory: two classes of attractors at work. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[49]  Laurence Mouchnino,et al.  Cortical facilitation of proprioceptive inputs related to gravitational balance constraints during step preparation. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  D. Wolpert,et al.  When Feeling Is More Important Than Seeing in Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2002, Current Biology.

[51]  R. Forget,et al.  The modulation of somatosensory evoked potentials during the foreperiod of a forewarned reaction time task. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[52]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Viewing the hand prior to movement improves accuracy of pointing performed toward the unseen contralateral hand , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  Jon Driver,et al.  The electrophysiology of tactile extinction: ERP correlates of unconscious somatosensory processing , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Heinrich H Bülthoff,et al.  Seeing the hand while reaching speeds up on‐line responses to a sudden change in target position , 2009, The Journal of physiology.

[55]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Reweighting visual cues by touch. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[56]  James L. Lyons,et al.  Goal-directed aiming: two components but multiple processes. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[57]  Gabriel Curio,et al.  Spatial attention related SEP amplitude modulations covary with BOLD signal in S1--a simultaneous EEG--fMRI study. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[58]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of adaptive sensory-motor control , 1986 .

[59]  H Spekreijse,et al.  The laplacian analysis of the pattern onset response in man. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[60]  Olivier Bertrand,et al.  Scalp Current Density Mapping: Value and Estimation from Potential Data , 1987, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[61]  Michelle Fleury,et al.  Visual stability with goal-directed eye and arm movements toward a target displaced during saccadic suppression , 1995, Psychological research.

[62]  Richard M. Leahy,et al.  Brainstorm: A User-Friendly Application for MEG/EEG Analysis , 2011, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[63]  J D Crawford,et al.  Spatial transformations for eye-hand coordination. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  Kazuhiko Seki,et al.  Gating of Sensory Input at Spinal and Cortical Levels during Preparation and Execution of Voluntary Movement , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[65]  C. Gross,et al.  Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. , 1994, Science.

[66]  David Whitaker,et al.  Preferential processing of tactile events under conditions of divided attention , 2009, Neuroreport.

[67]  J. Vercher,et al.  Internally driven control of reaching movements: A study on a proprioceptively deafferented subject , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.

[68]  A P Batista,et al.  Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates. , 1999, Science.

[69]  Ole Jensen,et al.  Parietal Oscillations Code Nonvisual Reach Targets Relative to Gaze and Body , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[70]  R. Sainburg,et al.  The effect of target modality on visual and proprioceptive contributions to the control of movement distance , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[71]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.7 July 2006 Special Issue: Probabilistic models of cognition Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control , 2022 .

[72]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[73]  Borís Burle,et al.  Direct evidence for cortical suppression of somatosensory afferents during visuomotor adaptation. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[74]  R. Romo,et al.  Periodicity and Firing Rate As Candidate Neural Codes for the Frequency of Vibrotactile Stimuli , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.