Salt-Induced Thirst Results in Increased Finickiness in Humans

Common sense suggests that water-deprived or food-deprived organisms should be more willing to consume foods or fluids that would be deemed undesirable under lower states of deprivation. With food, evidence favoring this account has been observed; however other studies find that hungry participants demonstrate increased finickiness—avoiding less palatable alternatives and consuming more of pleasant ones. This study set out to test whether thirst generally increases acceptability (the “common sense” view) or whether it produces increased finickiness. Thirst was induced in one group of participants (relative to nonthirsty controls), and the impact of this on their judgment of optimal and suboptimal water sources was examined. Thirsty participants liked optimal stimuli more, but liked suboptimal stimuli less, relative to controls, but this occurred only when the stimuli were actually tasted. These data suggest that thirst polarizes extant hedonic responses to fluids—finickiness—thereby maintaining optimal water selection under conditions of mild to moderate deprivation.

[1]  J. Glendinning,et al.  Is the bitter rejection response always adaptive? , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.

[2]  Individual differences in vulnerability to inescapable shock in rats. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[3]  N. Dess,et al.  Inescapable shock increases finickiness about drinking quinine-adulterated water in rats , 1988 .

[4]  C. Peter Herman,et al.  Hunger-induced finickiness in humans , 1995, Appetite.

[5]  R. Stevenson,et al.  Resistance to Interference of Olfactory Perceptual Learning , 2007 .

[6]  F. Strack,et al.  When hunger finds no fault with moldy corn: food deprivation reduces food-related disgust. , 2009, Emotion.

[7]  E. Dykens,et al.  Contaminated and unusual food combinations: what do people with Prader-Willi syndrome choose? , 2000, Mental retardation.

[8]  M. Changizi,et al.  Thirst Modulates a Perception , 2001, Perception.

[9]  M. Cabanac,et al.  Effets du repas sur l'agrément d'odeurs alimentaires et nonalimentaires chez l'homme , 1973 .

[10]  E. Rolls,et al.  Olfactory Sensory-Specific Satiety in Humans , 1997, Physiology & Behavior.

[11]  A. Raben,et al.  Effect of sensory perception of foods on appetite and food intake: a review of studies on humans , 2003, International Journal of Obesity.

[12]  J. Easterbrook The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. , 1959, Psychological review.

[13]  P. Lang,et al.  International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings (Tech. Rep. No. A-4) , 1999 .

[14]  E. Rolls,et al.  Thirst following water deprivation in humans. , 1980, The American journal of physiology.

[15]  A. L. Jones Shadow of the sun. , 1974, Science.

[16]  S. Schachter,et al.  Some extraordinary facts about obese humans and rats. , 1971, The American psychologist.

[17]  H. L. Jacobs,et al.  TASTE VERSUS CALORIES: SENSORY AND METABOLIC SIGNALS IN THE CONTROL OF FOOD INTAKE * , 1969, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  J. Altarriba,et al.  An Investigation of Sex Differences in Word Ratings Across Concrete, Abstract, and Emotion Words , 2008 .

[19]  J. Polivy,et al.  Palatability as a determinant of eating: Finickiness as a function of taste, hunger, and the prospect of good food. , 1990 .

[20]  R. Dantzer,et al.  The taste of sickness: Lipopolysaccharide-induced finickiness in rats , 2005, Physiology & Behavior.

[21]  Carmen Luciano,et al.  Verbal Regulation of Motivational States , 2006 .

[22]  Clark McCauley,et al.  Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors , 1994 .

[23]  W. Stewart,et al.  Effect of temperature and expectations on liking for beverages , 1988, Physiology & Behavior.