Using versus liking: Young children use ownership to predict actions but not to infer preferences.

Three experiments show that young children (N = 384) use ownership to predict actions but not to infer preferences. In Experiment 1, 3- to 6-year-olds considered ownership when predicting actions but did not expect it to trump preferences. In Experiment 2, 4- and 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, used ownership to predict actions, and 5-year-olds grasped that an agent would use his or her own property despite preferring someone else's. This experiment also showed that relating an agent to an object interfered with 3- and 4-year-olds' judgments that a more attractive object is preferred. Finally, Experiment 3 found that 3- and 4-year-olds do not believe that owning an object increases regard for it. These findings are informative about the kinds of information children use to predict actions and the inferences they make from ownership. The findings also reveal specificity in how children use ownership to make judgments about others, and suggest that children more closely relate ownership to people's actions than to their desires.

[1]  Qi Wang,et al.  A Comparison of American and Nepalese Children's Concepts of Freedom of Choice and Social Constraint , 2013, Cogn. Sci..

[2]  Daniel Brickman,et al.  The Influence of Competition on Children's Social Categories , 2011 .

[3]  Marjorie Rhodes,et al.  Naïve theories of social groups. , 2012, Child development.

[4]  Yarrow Dunham,et al.  Representing ‘Us’ and ‘Them’: Building Blocks of Intergroup Cognition , 2015 .

[5]  Meredith Render,et al.  The Concept of Property , 2017 .

[6]  S. Bosacki,et al.  Bridging theory of mind and the personal domain: children's reasoning about resistance to parental control. , 2010, Child development.

[7]  Borja Martinovic,et al.  First arrival and collective land ownership: How children reason about who owns the land , 2015 .

[8]  James K. Beggan On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. , 1992 .

[9]  Nicholaus S. Noles,et al.  Children Seek Historical Traces of Owned Objects. , 2016, Child development.

[10]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Anomalies: The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion, and Status Quo Bias , 1991 .

[11]  Madison L. Pesowski,et al.  Preschoolers and toddlers use ownership to predict basic emotions. , 2015, Emotion.

[12]  Ori Friedman,et al.  Children and Adults Use Gender and Age Stereotypes in Ownership Judgments , 2014 .

[13]  Charles W. Kalish,et al.  Children's Reasoning about Norms and Traits as Motives for Behavior. , 2004 .

[14]  Paul Bloom,et al.  Children prefer certain individuals over perfect duplicates , 2008, Cognition.

[15]  Susan A Gelman,et al.  You can't always want what you get: Children's intuitions about ownership and desire. , 2014, Cognitive development.

[16]  Shaylene E. Nancekivell,et al.  Mine, yours, no one's: children's understanding of how ownership affects object use. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[17]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children’s understanding of violations of property rights , 2011, Cognition.

[18]  B. Hood,et al.  Picture yourself: Self-focus and the endowment effect in preschool children , 2016, Cognition.

[19]  Susan A. Gelman,et al.  Young children’s preference for unique owned objects , 2016, Cognition.

[20]  J. Brehm Postdecision changes in the desirability of alternatives. , 1956, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[21]  Lise Vesterlund,et al.  Are Adults Better Behaved Than Children? Age, Experience, and the Endowment Effect. , 2001 .

[22]  C. Kalish,et al.  Children's ascriptions of property rights with changes of ownership , 2009 .

[23]  S. Carey,et al.  Consequences of "minimal" group affiliations in children. , 2011, Child development.

[24]  Margery Lucas,et al.  Fair game: The intuitive economics of resource exchange in four-year olds. , 2008 .

[25]  Madison L. Pesowski,et al.  Preschoolers use emotional reactions to infer relations: The case of ownership , 2016 .

[26]  J. Smetana Children's Impressions of Moral and Conventional Transgressors. , 1985 .

[27]  Ori Friedman,et al.  Preschoolers selectively infer history when explaining outcomes: evidence from explanations of ownership, liking, and use. , 2014, Child development.

[28]  Lisa Chalik,et al.  Preschoolers Use Social Allegiances to Predict Behavior , 2014 .

[29]  A. Shaw,et al.  Not by Strength Alone , 2015, Human nature.

[30]  Jeffrey Evans Stake,et al.  The property 'instinct'. , 2004, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[31]  Maykel Verkuyten,et al.  First arrival and owning the land: How children reason about ownership of territory , 2015 .

[32]  B. Hood,et al.  Young children's understanding of ownership rights for newly made objects , 2014 .

[33]  Nicholaus S. Noles,et al.  The nonobvious basis of ownership: preschool children trace the history and value of owned objects. , 2012, Child development.

[34]  Ori Friedman,et al.  Young children give priority to ownership when judging who should use an object. , 2014, Child development.

[35]  E. Turiel,et al.  Variation in Seriousness of Transgressions and Children's Moral and Conventional Concepts. , 1988 .

[36]  Stéphane Bernard,et al.  Rules Trump Desires in Preschoolers’ Predictions of Group Behavior , 2016 .

[37]  S. Carey,et al.  Constraints on the Acquisition of Social Category Concepts , 2014 .

[38]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Developing intuitions about free will between ages four and six , 2015, Cognition.