Effect of light adaptation on the perceptual red-green and yellow-blue opponent-color responses.

Spectral sensitivities of the red-green and yellow-blue opponent-color responses were determined under broad-band light adaptation for the light-adaptation levels of 5 to 5000 Td. With changing light-adaptation level, the spectral-sensitivity functions of the opponent-color systems change in shape, especially in the short-wavelength region of the spectrum. The light-adaptation effect on the red-green responses can be ascribed to the changes at the cone receptor level, whereas the light-adaptation effect on the yellow-blue responses can be ascribed to the changes at two sites, i.e., at the cone receptor site and at the opponent site.

[1]  J. D. Mollon,et al.  Post-receptoral adaptation , 1979, Vision Research.

[2]  W. Paulus,et al.  A new concept of retinal colour coding , 1983, Vision Research.

[3]  J. J. Vos,et al.  On the derivation of the foveal receptor primaries. , 1971, Vision research.

[4]  D. Jameson,et al.  Some Quantitative Aspects of an Opponent-Colors Theory. I. Chromatic Responses and Spectral Saturation , 1955 .

[5]  David H. Krantz,et al.  Opponent process additivity—II. Yellow/blue equilibria and nonlinear models , 1975, Vision Research.

[6]  Macular pigmentation and the spectral sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of macaques , 1978, Vision Research.

[7]  B. B. Lee,et al.  Light adaptation in cells of macaque lateral geniculate nucleus and its relation to human light adaptation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  Yoshimichi Ejima,et al.  Bezold-bru¨cke hue shift and nonlinearity in opponent-color process , 1984, Vision Research.

[9]  J S Werner,et al.  Opponent chromatic mechanisms: relation to photopigments and hue naming. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[10]  R. Marrocco Responses of monkey optic tract fibers to monochromatic lights. , 1972, Vision research.

[11]  Donald C. Hood,et al.  Detection and discrimination of small, brief lights: Variable tuning of opponent channels , 1984, Vision Research.

[12]  P. Gouras Identification of cone mechanisms in monkey ganglion cells , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[13]  C. R. Ingling,et al.  Orthogonal combination of the three visual channels , 1977, Vision Research.

[14]  E. Zrenner,et al.  Characteristics of the blue sensitive cone mechanism in primate retinal ganglion cells , 1981, Vision Research.

[15]  G. Buchsbaum,et al.  Trichromacy, opponent colours coding and optimum colour information transmission in the retina , 1983, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[16]  D. A. Burkhardt,et al.  Quantitative relations between color-opponent response of horizontal cells and action spectra of cones. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  P. Trezona,et al.  Rod participation in the 'blue' mechanism and its effect on colour matching. , 1970, Vision research.

[18]  C. R. Ingling The spectral sensitivity of the opponent-color channels , 1977, Vision Research.

[19]  J. Mollon,et al.  A theory of theΠ1 andΠ3 color mechanisms of stiles , 1979, Vision Research.

[20]  C. M. Cicerone,et al.  Opponent-process additivity--I: red-green equilibria. , 1974, Vision research.

[21]  S. M. Luria,et al.  Color-mixture functions at low luminance levels. , 1964, Vision research.

[22]  B. Drum Short-wavelength cones contribute to achromatic sensitivity , 1983, Vision Research.

[23]  T. Cornsweet,et al.  The staircrase-method in psychophysics. , 1962, The American journal of psychology.

[24]  J. Werner,et al.  Short-wave cone input to the red-green opponent channel , 1979, Vision Research.

[25]  R. Harwerth,et al.  Red-Green Cone Interactions in the Increment-Threshold Spectral Sensitivity of Primates , 1971, Science.

[26]  D. Macleod,et al.  Blue-sensitive cones do not contribute to luminance. , 1980, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[27]  David H. Krantz,et al.  Opponent-process additivity—III Effect of moderate chromatic adaptation , 1975, Vision Research.

[28]  Y. Ejima,et al.  Spatial properties of red-green and yellow-blue perceptual opponent-color response , 1984, Vision Research.

[29]  D JAMESON,et al.  A psychophysical study of white. III. Adaptation as variant. , 1951, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[30]  P. Gouras,et al.  Functional properties of ganglion cells of the rhesus monkey retina. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[31]  J. Bowmaker,et al.  Visual pigments of rods and cones in a human retina. , 1980, The Journal of physiology.

[32]  F. M. de Monasterio,et al.  Signals from blue cones in “red-green” opponent-colour ganglion cells of the macaque retina , 1979, Vision Research.

[33]  D. Hood,et al.  Interactions between rod and cone channels above threshold: A test of various models , 1982, Vision Research.

[34]  D. Jameson,et al.  An opponent-process theory of color vision. , 1957, Psychological review.

[35]  C. R. Ingling,et al.  The relationship between spectral sensitivity and spatial sensitivity for the primate r-g X-channel , 1983, Vision Research.

[36]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm , 1975, Vision Research.

[37]  R. Normann,et al.  The effects of background illumination on the photoresponses of red and green cones. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.

[38]  R. M. Boynton,et al.  Chromatic border perception: The role of red- and green-sensitive cones , 1978, Vision Research.

[39]  G Westheimer,et al.  The Maxwellian view. , 1966, Vision research.

[40]  R. Massof,et al.  Vector model for normal and dichromatic color vision. , 1980, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[41]  A. Valberg,et al.  A method for the precise determination of achromatic colours including white. , 1971, Vision research.