Does good science make good medicine? Incorporating evidence into practice is complicated by the fact that clinical practice is as much art as science.

LES TENDANCES DE LA PRATIQUE DES MÉDECINS varient considérablement, comme le démontrent McAlister et ses collaborateurs en ce qui concerne la prise en charge de l’hypertension (page 23). On a mis de l’avant de nombreuses raisons pour expliquer cette variation et l’écart par rapport aux données probantes scientifiques. L’auteur est d’avis que les solutions doivent tenir compte de la nature des preuves scientifiques, du rôle de la science dans la pratique clinique, du rôle du jugement, de l’autorité professionnelle et du besoin d’éducation médicale continue. La pratique diffère de la science : la science sert, mais son application à chaque patient en particulier est fonction du jugement du médecin. L’analyse de la variation dans la pratique devrait tenir compte de la formation initiale du médecin et de son acquisition continue du savoir, de l’absence de mécanisme solide d’appui et d’examen critique par les pairs et de l’abus de l’autorité du médecin.

[1]  D M Eddy,et al.  Variations in physician practice: the role of uncertainty. , 1984, Health affairs.

[2]  R. Haynes,et al.  A taxonomy and critical review of tested strategies for the application of clinical practice recommendations: from "official" to "individual" clinical policy. , 1988, American journal of preventive medicine.

[3]  K. Malterud,et al.  The legitimacy of clinical knowledge: Towards a medical epistemology embracing the art of medicine , 1995, Theoretical medicine.

[4]  D. Sackett,et al.  The Ends of Human Life: Medical Ethics in a Liberal Polity , 1992, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[5]  Practice Variation: Learned and Socio-Economic Factors , 1990, Advances in dental research.

[6]  R. Haynes,et al.  Evidence for the effectiveness of CME. A review of 50 randomized controlled trials. , 1992, JAMA.

[7]  William A. Knaus,et al.  A controlled trial to improve care for seriously ill hospitalized patients. The study to understand prognoses and preferences for outcomes and risks of treatments (SUPPORT). The SUPPORT Principal Investigators. , 1995, JAMA.

[8]  A. Macintyre,et al.  Toward a theory of medical fallibility. , 1975, The Hastings Center report.

[9]  A L Greer,et al.  The two cultures of biomedicine: can there be consensus? , 1987, JAMA.

[10]  R. Haynes,et al.  A critical appraisal of the efficacy of continuing medical education. , 1984, JAMA.

[11]  M. Enkin,et al.  Do practice guidelines guide practice? The effect of a consensus statement on the practice of physicians. , 1989, The New England journal of medicine.

[12]  R. Blais,et al.  Variations in surgical rates in Quebec: does access to teaching hospitals make a difference? , 1993, CMAJ : Canadian Medical Association journal = journal de l'Association medicale canadienne.

[13]  R H Brook,et al.  Does inappropriate use explain geographic variations in the use of health care services? A study of three procedures. , 1987, JAMA.

[14]  C. Naylor Grey zones of clinical practice: some limits to evidence-based medicine , 1995, The Lancet.

[15]  David M. Eddy,et al.  Clinical decision making: from theory to practice. Cost-effectiveness analysis. A conversation with my father. , 1992, JAMA.