Economic demand and essential value.

The strength of a rat's eating reflex correlates with hunger level when strength is measured by the response frequency that precedes eating (B. F. Skinner, 1932a, 1932b). On the basis of this finding, Skinner argued response frequency could index reflex strength. Subsequent work documented difficulties with this notion because responding was affected not only by the strengthening properties of the reinforcer but also by the rate-shaping effects of the schedule. This article obviates this problem by measuring strength via methods from behavioral economics. This approach uses demand curves to map how reinforcer consumption changes with changes in the "price" different ratio schedules impose. An exponential equation is used to model these demand curves. The value of this exponential's rate constant is used to scale the strength or essential value of a reinforcer, independent of the scalar dimensions of the reinforcer. Essential value determines the consumption level to be expected at particular prices and the response level that will occur to support that consumption. This approach permits comparing reinforcers that differ in kind, contributing toward the goal of scaling reinforcer value.

[1]  S. Hursh,et al.  Substitution and caloric regulation in a closed economy. , 1996, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[2]  P. Killeen Economics, ecologics, and mechanics: The dynamics of responding under conditions of varying motivation. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[3]  A. Silberberg,et al.  Inferior-good and giffen-good effects in monkey choice behavior , 1987 .

[4]  S. Hursh,et al.  The economics of drug abuse: a quantitative assessment of drug demand. , 2005, Molecular interventions.

[5]  G. S. Reynolds,et al.  A quantitative analysis of the responding maintained by interval schedules of reinforcement. , 1968, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[6]  R J HERRNSTEIN,et al.  Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. , 1961, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[7]  S R Hursh,et al.  Relative reinforcing strength of three N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonists with different onsets of action. , 2002, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.

[8]  J. Falk Physiology and Behavior. , 1973 .

[9]  S R Hursh,et al.  The economics of daily consumption controlling food- and water-reinforced responding. , 1978, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[10]  D Shurtleff,et al.  A cost-benefit analysis of demand for food. , 1988, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[11]  F. J. Sodetz,et al.  Reduction of heroin intake in baboons by an economic constraint , 1980, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[12]  G. Madden,et al.  Similar consumption and responding across single and multiple sources of drug. , 1999, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[13]  G. Galbicka,et al.  Assessing the Value of Television as Environmental Enrichment for Individually Housed Rhesus Monkeys: A Behavioral Economic Approach. , 1999, Contemporary topics in laboratory animal science.

[14]  W HODOS,et al.  Progressive Ratio as a Measure of Reward Strength , 1961, Science.

[15]  B. Skinner Drive and Reflex Strength , 1932 .

[16]  S R Hursh,et al.  Economic concepts for the analysis of behavior. , 1980, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[17]  W. Hodos,et al.  Effects of increment size and reinforcer volume on progressive ratio performance. , 1963, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[18]  Gerhard Tintner,et al.  Mathematical Analysis for Economists. , 1938 .

[19]  W. Bickel,et al.  Replacing relative reinforcing efficacy with behavioral economic demand curves. , 2006, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[20]  G. Madden,et al.  The Price of Change: The Behavioral Economics of Drug Dependence , 1998 .

[21]  S. Hursh,et al.  Electrical brain stimulation and food reinforcement dissociated by demand elasticity , 1981, Physiology & Behavior.

[22]  C E Sumpter,et al.  Predicting and scaling hens' preferences for topographically different responses. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[23]  W. Bickel,et al.  Modeling drug consumption in the clinic using simulation procedures: demand for heroin and cigarettes in opioid-dependent outpatients. , 1999, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[24]  T. Foster,et al.  The demand for straw and feathers as litter substrates by laying hens , 2000 .

[25]  W Temple,et al.  The effects of differing response types and price manipulations on demand measures. , 1999, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[26]  M. Dawkins From an animal's point of view: Motivation, fitness, and animal welfare , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[27]  S. Hursh,et al.  Methadone and nicotine self-administration in humans: a behavioral economic analysis , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[28]  W. Bickel,et al.  Deconstructing relative reinforcing efficacy and situating the measures of pharmacological reinforcement with behavioral economics: a theoretical proposal , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[29]  H. L. Miller Matching-based hedonic scaling in the pigeon. , 1976, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[30]  J A Nevin,et al.  An integrative model for the study of behavioral momentum. , 1992, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[31]  J. Rowlett A labor-supply analysis of cocaine self-administration under progressive-ratio schedules: antecedents, methodologies, and perspectives , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[32]  J. Holst,et al.  Experimental and clinical pharmacology , 2008 .

[33]  C. B. Ferster,et al.  Schedules of reinforcement , 1957 .

[34]  B. Skinner,et al.  Principles of Behavior , 1944 .

[35]  W K Bickel,et al.  Behavioral economics of human drug self‐administration: progressive ratio versus random sequences of response requirements , 2001, Behavioural pharmacology.

[36]  W M Baum,et al.  Choice as time allocation. , 1969, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[37]  B. Skinner Drive and Reflex Strength: II , 1932 .

[38]  M. Branch,et al.  Effects of step size and break-point criterion on progressive-ratio performance. , 1998, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[39]  S. Hursh,et al.  Relative reinforcing effects of three opioids with different durations of action. , 2002, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.

[40]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Advances in behavioral economics , 2004 .

[41]  W. Timberlake,et al.  Effects of Reward Magnitude on Key Pecking and Eating by Pigeons in a Closed Economy , 1984 .

[42]  G. Madden,et al.  Three predictions of the economic concept of unit price in a choice context. , 2000, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[43]  S. Hursh,et al.  The Quantitative Analysis of Economic Behavior With Laboratory Animals , 1989 .

[44]  S R Hursh,et al.  Normalized demand for drugs and other reinforcers. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[45]  R. Herrnstein On the law of effect. , 1970, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[46]  Stephen E. G. Lea,et al.  The psychology and economics of demand. , 1978 .

[47]  J. Nevin,et al.  Variable-ratio versus variable-interval schedules: response rate, resistance to change, and preference. , 2001, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[48]  J. M. Dake,et al.  Labor supply and consumption of food in a closed economy under a range of fixed- and random-ratio schedules: tests of unit price. , 2005, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[49]  Marian Stamp Dawkins,et al.  Battery hens name their price: Consumer demand theory and the measurement of ethological ‘needs’ , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[50]  M. Lesage,et al.  Progressive-ratio schedules of drug delivery in the analysis of drug self-administration: a review , 1998, Psychopharmacology.

[51]  K. Lattal,et al.  Reward Density And Variable-interval Schedule Performance In An Open Economy. , 1999, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[52]  J. Nevin,et al.  Preference and resistance to change with constant-duration schedule components. , 2000, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[53]  B. Skinner The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. , 1931 .

[54]  Shin Tsunematsu Effort- and time-cost effects on demand curves for food by pigeons under short session closed economies , 2001, Behavioural Processes.

[55]  S. Hursh Behavioral economics of drug self-administration and drug abuse policy. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[56]  R. Bauman,et al.  An experimental analysis of the cost of food in a closed economy. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.