Affect and letter-writing: Unconventional conventions in casual writing by young Japanese women

With special attention to the use of unconventional practices and pictorial signs in casual letter writing, this article shows how young Japanese women effectively exploit affect-laden shape, form, and function in order to establish intimate and solidary relationships. They rely on both conventional and unconventional aspects of Japanese orthography, encoding affect specific to the given context and merging spoken with written modes of self-representation. In so doing, they seem to draw on diverse "frames" of written language, and to manipulate symbolic means of association and integration for achieving reciprocity. These features not only provide the basis for reciprocity, but may also suggest a new mode of literacy caused by social change. (Writing, literacy, affect, Japanese, pictorial signs, letter-writing)* Increasingly attracting linguists' attention is the interface between language and affect, which has been a neglected area of investigation in linguistics. As Irvine 1990 summarizes, many linguists formerly assumed that affectivity only pertains to the individual and is largely a property of parole rather than of langue. They assumed also that the affective function is a non-referential dimension of language. Thus the affective function was not part of linguistic competence and was marginal to linguistic study. This view was later criticized, and the concept of competence was reasonably adjusted to include "poetic" functions (Jakobson 1960, Hymes 1966) and non-referential functions of communication (Silverstein 1976) into the study of language. Recently, even more linguists have come to appreciate the grammatical realization of affect in language. Data suggest that discourse is saturated with affect-carrying devices such as word order, verb voice, pronominal reference, mood, tense/aspect, and case/number/gender/animacy marking (Ochs & Schieffelin 1989). Sapir (1921:38-41) and Jakobson (1960, 1990:99-102) both claimed that affect and emotion are fundamental elements of human life, and are mani? 1997 Cambridge University Press 0047-4045/97 $7.50 + .10 103 This content downloaded from 157.55.39.191 on Tue, 11 Oct 2016 05:21:12 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

[1]  D. Tannen Coherence in Spoken and Written Discourse , 1984 .

[2]  Lesley Milroy,et al.  Language and social networks , 1980 .

[3]  Human motives and cultural models: The directive force of morality tales in a Mexican community , 1992 .

[4]  Klaus R. Scherer,et al.  Emotional experience in cultural context: A comparison between Europe, Japan, and the United States. , 1988 .

[5]  P. Garvin A Prague school reader on esthetics, literary structure, and style , 1955 .

[6]  J. Marshall Unger,et al.  The fifth generation fallacy , 1988, The Journal of Asian Studies.

[7]  G. Lakoff,et al.  More than Cool Reason: A Field Guide to Poetic Metaphor , 1991 .

[8]  Bambi B. Schieffelin,et al.  Language has a heart , 1989 .

[9]  H. Haarmann,et al.  Symbolic Values of Foreign Language Use: From the Japanese Case to a General Sociolinguistic Perspective , 1989 .

[10]  D. Tannen,et al.  The Relation Between Written and Spoken Language , 1987 .

[11]  M. Halliday Language as social semiotic: The social interpretation of language and meaning , 1976 .

[12]  M. Nystrand A Social-Interactive Model of Writing , 1989 .

[13]  P. Trudgill Sex, covert prestige and linguistic change in the urban British English of Norwich , 1972, Language in Society.

[14]  Samuel Leinhardt,et al.  Perspectives on social network research , 1980 .

[15]  T. Sebeok,et al.  Style in language , 1962 .

[16]  N. Besnier Literacy and the Notion of Person on Nukulaelae Atoll , 1991 .

[17]  Susan D. Lima,et al.  The linguistics of literacy , 1992 .

[18]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Metaphors We Live By , 1980 .

[19]  F. Corblin,et al.  Discourse structure and anaphora: Barbara Fox, Written and conversational English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1987. 173 pp. $34.50. , 1989 .

[20]  R. Jakobson On Language , 1990 .

[21]  K. Lorenz Studies in animal and human behaviour , 1970 .

[22]  Domination and the Arts of Resistance , 1991 .

[23]  Geoffrey Nunberg,et al.  Transfers of Meaning , 1995, J. Semant..

[24]  Catherine Lutz,et al.  Language and the Politics of Emotion , 1990 .

[25]  P. Willis Learning to Labor: How Working-Class Kids Get Working-Class Jobs , 1976 .

[26]  Claudia Strauss,et al.  Human Motives and Cultural Models , 1992 .

[27]  Jack Goody,et al.  The interface between the written and the oral , 1989 .

[28]  N. Quinn Cultural models in language and thought: Convergent evidence for a cultural model of American marriage , 1987 .

[29]  A. Wierzbicka Human Emotions: Universal or Culture-Specific? , 1986 .

[30]  Wallace L. Chafe The Flow of Ideas in a Sample of Written Language , 1992 .

[31]  G. Lakoff,et al.  The cognitive model of anger inherent in American English , 1983 .

[32]  M. Silverstein Shifters, linguistic categories, and cultural description , 1976 .

[33]  R. Jakobson Linguistics and poetics , 1960 .

[34]  Penelope Brown,et al.  Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage , 1989 .

[35]  William Frawley,et al.  The development of oral and written language in social contexts , 1986 .

[36]  H. Markus,et al.  Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. , 1991 .

[37]  Deborah Tannen,et al.  Spoken and Written Language , 1982 .

[38]  J. Smith,et al.  Variability in Written Japanese: Towards a Sociolinguistics of Script Choice. , 1996 .

[39]  Mark S. Granovetter The Strength of Weak Ties , 1973, American Journal of Sociology.

[40]  B. Street Cross-Cultural Approaches to Literacy , 1993 .

[41]  N. Besnier,et al.  The Linguistic Relationships of Spoken and Written Nukulaelae , 1988 .

[42]  George Lakoff,et al.  Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things , 1987 .

[43]  S. Thompson,et al.  Discourse description : diverse linguistic analyses of a fund-raising text , 1992 .

[44]  Rosalind Horowitz,et al.  Comprehending Oral and Written Language. , 1987 .

[45]  A. Ortony,et al.  What's basic about basic emotions? , 1990, Psychological review.

[46]  K. Basso,et al.  Meaning in Anthropology , 1976 .

[47]  P. Eckert Adolescent social structure and the spread of linguistic change , 1988, Language in Society.

[48]  N. Besnier,et al.  Literacy and feelings: The encoding of affect in Nukulaelae letters , 1989 .

[49]  D. Tannen Spoken and written language : exploring orality and literacy , 1984 .

[50]  P. Ekman,et al.  The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. , 1994 .

[51]  Catherine Lutz,et al.  Cultural models in language and thought: Goals, events, and understanding in Ifaluk emotion theory , 1987 .

[52]  Kuniyoshi Kataoka Affect in Japanese Women's Letter Writing: Use of Sentence-Final Particles ne and yo and Orthographic Conventions , 1995 .

[53]  D. Biber Spoken and Written Textual Dimensions in English: Resolving the Contradictory Findings , 1986 .

[54]  R. Bauman,et al.  Genre, Intertextuality, and Social Power , 1992 .

[55]  W. Chafe,et al.  Properties of spoken and written language. , 1987 .

[56]  J. T. Irvine,et al.  Registering affect: heteroglossia in the linguistic expression of emotion , 1990 .

[57]  Everett M. Rogers,et al.  CHAPTER 8 – NETWORK ANALYSIS OF THE DIFFUSION OF INNOVATIONS1 , 1979 .

[58]  D. Holland,et al.  Cultural models in language and thought: An appraisal , 1987 .

[59]  P. Ekman Are there basic emotions? , 1992, Psychological review.

[60]  William F. Hanks,et al.  discourse genres in a theory of practice , 1987 .