The precedence effect for noise bursts of different bandwidths. II. Comparison of model algorithms

In this investigation, different model algorithms were tested for their ability to simulate the precedence effect for ongoing (non-impulsive) noise-bursts of different bandwidths (100 Hz, 400 Hz and 800 Hz). The psychoacoustical reference data—in which the perceived lateral position of a noise burst (200-ms duration, 20-ms cos 2 -ramps, 500-Hz center frequency) in the presence of one reflection (inter-stimulus interval: 0.0 ms-0.4 ms) was determined—were taken from a preceding paper on this investigation. It is shown that models which simulate the precedence effect by using the special characteristics of the auditory periphery or by focusing on the spectral dominance region fail when stimuli of longer duration than clicks are used, while a modified Lindemann model still shows satisfactory results. Furthermore, it was found that the trading ratio of ITDs and ILDs can be assumed to be constant for the stimuli tested. A discounting of ITD cues, as was found by Rakerd and Hartmann (1985), was not observed for the type of stimuli tested here. Instead, a discounting of the precedence effect occurred for some of the listeners when the bandwidth of the signals was very narrow. In the model simulation, it was not necessary to consider cross-frequency-band interaction like the second coincidence weighting of Stern et al. (1988), and it was sufficient to estimate the average of the outputs of the involved frequency bands.

[1]  R. Patterson,et al.  Time-domain modeling of peripheral auditory processing: a modular architecture and a software platform. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  W. Lindemann Extension of a binaural cross-correlation model by contralateral inhibition. I. Simulation of lateralization for stationary signals. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  Jonas Braasch,et al.  Localization in the presence of a distracter and reverberation in the frontal horizontal plane. II. Model algorithms , 2002 .

[4]  W. Gaik,et al.  Combined evaluation of interaural time and intensity differences: psychoacoustic results and computer modeling. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  W. Hartmann,et al.  Localization of sound in rooms, II: The effects of a single reflecting surface. , 1985, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  A. Kohlrausch,et al.  Binaural processing model based on contralateral inhibition. I. Model structure. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  Klaus Hartung,et al.  Localization in the Presence of a Distracter and Reverberation in the Frontal Horizontal Plane. I. Psychoacoustical Data , 2002 .

[8]  Jie Huang,et al.  Echo avoidance in a computational model of the precedence effect , 1999, Speech Commun..

[9]  R. Meddis Simulation of mechanical to neural transduction in the auditory receptor. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  Patrick M. Zurek,et al.  The Precedence Effect , 1987 .

[11]  J. Braasch Localization in the Presence of a Distracter and Reverberation in the Frontal Horizontal Plane: III. The Role of Interaural Level Differences , 2003 .

[12]  C Trahiotis,et al.  Peripheral auditory processing and investigations of the "precedence effect" which utilize successive transient stimuli. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  J. Raatgever On the binaural processing of stimuli with different interaural phase relations , 1980 .

[14]  Daniel J. Tollin,et al.  COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF THE LATERALISATION OF CLICKS AND THEIR ECHOES , 1998 .

[15]  R Meddis,et al.  Simulation of auditory-neural transduction: further studies. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  Jonas Braasch,et al.  The precedence effect for noise bursts of different bandwidths. I. Psychoacoustical data , 2003 .

[17]  W. Lindemann Extension of a binaural cross-correlation model by contralateral inhibition. II. The law of the first wave front. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  M. Reed,et al.  A model for the computation and encoding of azimuthal information by the lateral superior olive. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  G. Henning,et al.  Some aspects of the lateralization of echoed sound in man. II. The role of the stimulus spectrum. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  R. Meddis,et al.  Implementation details of a computation model of the inner hair‐cell auditory‐nerve synapse , 1990 .

[21]  A. Zeiberg,et al.  Lateralization of complex binaural stimuli: a weighted-image model. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.