Association of Childhood Blood Lead Levels With Cognitive Function and Socioeconomic Status at Age 38 Years and With IQ Change and Socioeconomic Mobility Between Childhood and Adulthood

Importance Many children in the United States and around the world are exposed to lead, a developmental neurotoxin. The long-term cognitive and socioeconomic consequences of lead exposure are uncertain. Objective To test the hypothesis that childhood lead exposure is associated with cognitive function and socioeconomic status in adulthood and with changes in IQ and socioeconomic mobility between childhood and midlife. Design, Setting, and Participants A prospective cohort study based on a population-representative 1972-1973 birth cohort from New Zealand; the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study observed participants to age 38 years (until December 2012). Exposures Childhood lead exposure ascertained as blood lead levels measured at age 11 years. High blood lead levels were observed among children from all socioeconomic status levels in this cohort. Main Outcomes and Measures The IQ (primary outcome) and indexes of Verbal Comprehension, Perceptual Reasoning, Working Memory, and Processing Speed (secondary outcomes) were assessed at age 38 years using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–IV (WAIS-IV; IQ range, 40-160). Socioeconomic status (primary outcome) was assessed at age 38 years using the New Zealand Socioeconomic Index-2006 (NZSEI-06; range, 10 [lowest]-90 [highest]). Results Of 1037 original participants, 1007 were alive at age 38 years, of whom 565 (56%) had been lead tested at age 11 years (54% male; 93% white). Mean (SD) blood lead level at age 11 years was 10.99 (4.63) µg/dL. Among blood-tested participants included at age 38 years, mean WAIS-IV score was 101.16 (14.82) and mean NZSEI-06 score was 49.75 (17.12). After adjusting for maternal IQ, childhood IQ, and childhood socioeconomic status, each 5-µg/dL higher level of blood lead in childhood was associated with a 1.61-point lower score (95% CI, −2.48 to −0.74) in adult IQ, a 2.07-point lower score (95% CI, −3.14 to −1.01) in perceptual reasoning, and a 1.26-point lower score (95% CI, −2.38 to −0.14) in working memory. Associations of childhood blood lead level with deficits in verbal comprehension and processing speed were not statistically significant. After adjusting for confounders, each 5-µg/dL higher level of blood lead in childhood was associated with a 1.79-unit lower score (95% CI, −3.17 to −0.40) in socioeconomic status. An association between greater blood lead levels and a decline in IQ and socioeconomic status from childhood to adulthood was observed with 40% of the association with downward mobility mediated by cognitive decline from childhood. Conclusions and Relevance In this cohort born in New Zealand in 1972-1973, childhood lead exposure was associated with lower cognitive function and socioeconomic status at age 38 years and with declines in IQ and with downward social mobility. Childhood lead exposure may have long-term ramifications.

[1]  John Sarisky,et al.  Blood Lead Levels Among Children Aged <6 Years - Flint, Michigan, 2013-2016. , 2016, MMWR. Morbidity and mortality weekly report.

[2]  B. W. Pigozzi,et al.  The Racial Gap in Childhood Blood Lead Levels Related to Socioeconomic Position of Residence in Metropolitan Detroit , 2016 .

[3]  Richie Poulton,et al.  The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study: overview of the first 40 years, with an eye to the future , 2015, Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology.

[4]  Educational Services for Children Affected by Lead Expert Panel Educational interventions for children affected by lead , 2015 .

[5]  R. Poulton,et al.  Community Water Fluoridation and Intelligence: Prospective Study in New Zealand. , 2015, American journal of public health.

[6]  J. Schwartz,et al.  Lead exposure and rate of change in cognitive function in older women. , 2014, Environmental research.

[7]  Maitreyi Mazumdar,et al.  Low-level environmental lead exposure in childhood and adult intellectual function: a follow-up study , 2011, Environmental health : a global access science source.

[8]  R. Renner Out of Plumb: When Water Treatment Causes Lead Contamination , 2009, Environmental health perspectives.

[9]  Vincent J Schmithorst,et al.  Altered myelination and axonal integrity in adults with childhood lead exposure: a diffusion tensor imaging study. , 2009, Neurotoxicology.

[10]  T. Glass,et al.  Cumulative Lead Dose and Cognitive Function in Older Adults , 2009, Epidemiology.

[11]  Mekibib Altaye,et al.  Decreased Brain Volume in Adults with Childhood Lead Exposure , 2008, PLoS medicine.

[12]  D. Bellinger,et al.  Very low lead exposures and children's neurodevelopment , 2008, Current opinion in pediatrics.

[13]  N. Wilson,et al.  Lessons from the removal of lead from gasoline for controlling other environmental pollutants: A case study from New Zealand , 2008, Environmental health : a global access science source.

[14]  B. Schwartz,et al.  Cumulative Lead Dose and Cognitive Function in Adults: A Review of Studies That Measured Both Blood Lead and Bone Lead , 2006, Environmental health perspectives.

[15]  Herbert L. Needleman,et al.  Low-Level Environmental Lead Exposure and Children’s Intellectual Function: An International Pooled Analysis , 2005, Environmental health perspectives.

[16]  Daniel J. Flannery,et al.  Outcomes in young adulthood for very-low-birth-weight infants. , 2002, The New England journal of medicine.

[17]  D. .,et al.  Outcomes in Young Adulthood for Very-Low-BirthWeight Infants , 2002 .

[18]  M. Kolczak,et al.  Neurotoxicity in young adults 20 years after childhood exposure to lead: the Bunker Hill experience. , 1998, Occupational and environmental medicine.

[19]  K. Flegal,et al.  Blood lead levels in the US population. Phase 1 of the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III, 1988 to 1991) , 1994, JAMA.

[20]  Roberta F. White,et al.  Residual cognitive deficits 50 years after lead poisoning during childhood. , 1993, British journal of industrial medicine.

[21]  A. Caspi,et al.  The natural history of change in intellectual performance: who changes? How much? Is it meaningful? , 1993, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[22]  E. Silbergeld,et al.  The long-term effects of exposure to low doses of lead in childhood. , 2010, The New England journal of medicine.

[23]  D Bellinger,et al.  The long-term effects of exposure to low doses of lead in childhood. An 11-year follow-up report. , 1990, The New England journal of medicine.

[24]  Phil A. Silva,et al.  Blood lead, intelligence, reading attainment, and behaviour in eleven year old children in Dunedin, New Zealand. , 1988, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[25]  P. Silva,et al.  Blood lead levels in Dunedin 11 year old children. , 1986, The New Zealand medical journal.

[26]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  P. Desilva Blood lead levels and the haematocrit correction. , 1984, The Annals of occupational hygiene.