Identification of stimuli and input pathways mediating food-attraction conditioning in the snail, Helix

Snails become conditioned by a single feeding episode to locate foods which they were unable to locate prior to feeding. To identify which of the different stimulus parameters of the food mediate learning, snails were presented with isolated stimulus components during feeding and re-tested the next day for their ability to locate the food. None of the individual components was sufficient to promote conditioning. Odor combined with a bulk stimulus conditioned the animals, as indicated by their subsequently locating the food. Elimination of the olfactory sensory inputs from the anterior and/or posterior tentacles prior to conditioning revealed that the acquisition of the olfactory memory requires olfactory stimulation of the sensory epithelia on the anterior tentacles. Recall of memory during olfactory orientation requires functional epithelia on the posterior tentacles, which suggests that the same odor is processed by different input pathways under different situations. Animals with the olfactory epithelia functional on the same side during conditioning and food searching were able to locate the conditioned food. Animals with different epithelia functional during conditioning and food searching failed, which suggests that olfactory memory is stored within one side of the nervous system and cannot be accessed from the contralateral side.

[1]  R. Menzel,et al.  Learning Experiments on the Use of Side — Specific Information in the Olfactory and Visual System in the Honey Bee (Apis mellifica) , 1972 .

[2]  R. Hudson Olfactory imprinting , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[3]  R. Wehner,et al.  Does interocular transfer occur in visual navigation by ants? , 1985, Nature.

[4]  E. Kandel Cellular basis of behavior: An introduction to behavioral neurobiology. , 1976 .

[5]  D. Tank,et al.  Odour-modulated collective network oscillations of olfactory interneurons in a terrestrial mollusc , 1990, Nature.

[6]  A. Gelperin,et al.  Vital staining from dye-coated microprobes identifies new olfactory interneurons for optical and electrical recording , 1997, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[7]  W. G. Hall,et al.  Appetitive learning in 1-day-old rat pups. , 1979, Science.

[8]  J Atema,et al.  Chemoreception in the sea: adaptations of chemoreceptors and behaviour to aquatic stimulus conditions. , 1985, Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology.

[9]  R. Chase,et al.  Tracing neural pathways in snail olfaction: From the tip of the tentacles to the brain and beyond , 1993, Microscopy research and technique.

[10]  R. Chase Lessons from snail tentacles , 1986 .

[11]  Alan Gelperin,et al.  A Taste for Learning , 1990 .

[12]  Behavioural and Electrical Responses of the Land Snail, Ezohelix flexibilis (Fulton), to Odours (With 5 Text-figures and 7 Tables) , 1967 .

[13]  F. Krasne,et al.  What We Can Learn from Invertebrate Learning , 1995 .

[14]  J. Krebs,et al.  Memory for the location of stored food in marsh tits , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  R. Chase,et al.  Responses to odors mapped in snail tentacle and brain by [14C]-2- deoxyglucose autoradiography , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  M. Leon The neurobiology of filial learning. , 1992, Annual review of psychology.

[17]  J Atema,et al.  Eddy Chemotaxis and Odor Landscapes: Exploration of Nature With Animal Sensors. , 1996, The Biological bulletin.