The furrow illusion: peripheral motion becomes aligned with stationary contours.

A spot moved vertically up and down across a background grating that was tilted at 45°. In foveal vision this was seen accurately, but when viewed peripherally the spot's path was perceptually attracted toward the grating orientation, and at large eccentricities (>20°) the spot appeared to move at 45°, parallel to the grating. The intersections between the grating and the moving spot drive this illusion, revealing profound differences between fovea and periphery in processing visual motion.

[1]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Eyes Pursue Moving Objects, Not Retinal Motion Signals , 2010, Perception.

[2]  Chang-Bing Huang,et al.  Transitions between Central and Peripheral Vision Create Spatial/Temporal Distortions: A Hypothesis Concerning the Perceived Break of the Curveball , 2010, PloS one.

[3]  S Anstis,et al.  Picturing Peripheral Acuity , 1998, Perception.

[4]  Colin Blakemore,et al.  Interactions between orientations in human vision , 1973, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  Sieu K. Khuu,et al.  The role of motion streaks in the perception of the kinetic Zollner illusion. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[6]  S. Anstis The Flash-Lag Effect during Illusory Chopstick Rotation , 2007, Perception.

[7]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Factors affecting footsteps: contrast can change the apparent speed, amplitude and direction of motion , 2004, Vision Research.

[8]  S Anstis,et al.  Footsteps and Inchworms: Illusions Show That Contrast Affects Apparent Speed , 2001, Perception.

[9]  O. Braddick Segmentation versus integration in visual motion processing , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[10]  J. Zanker,et al.  The Directional Tuning of the Barber-Pole Illusion , 2001, Perception.

[11]  R. Blake,et al.  Misdirected visual motion in the peripheral visual field , 1992, Vision Research.

[12]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Contrast-reversing global-motion stimuli reveal local interactions between first- and second-order motion signals , 2004, Vision Research.

[13]  H. Ono,et al.  Factors Influencing the Apparent Rotation of a Line on Radially Transformed Backgrounds , 1983, Perception.

[14]  M T Swanston,et al.  Displacement of the path of perceived movement by intersection with static contours , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  P. Wenderoth,et al.  Relationships between the kinetic, alternating-line, and Poggendorff illusions: The effects of interstimulus interval, inducing parallels, and fixation , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  J. Lorenceau,et al.  Directional shifts in the barber pole illusion: Effects of spatial frequency, spatial adaptation, and lateral masking , 2006, Visual Neuroscience.

[17]  S M Anstis,et al.  Letter: A chart demonstrating variations in acuity with retinal position. , 1974, Vision research.

[18]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. , 1991, Vision research.

[19]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Moving objects appear to slow down at low contrasts , 2003, Neural Networks.

[20]  P. Thompson Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast , 1982, Vision Research.

[21]  C. Curcio,et al.  Topography of ganglion cells in human retina , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.