Standing out and standing in: The psychology of control in America and Japan.

There are at least two general paths to a feeling of control. In primary control, individuals enhance their rewards by influencing existing realities (e.g., other people, circumstances, symptoms, or behavior problems). In secondary control, individuals enhance their rewards by accommodating to existing realities and maximizing satisfaction or goodness of fit with things as they are. American psychologists have written extensively about control, but have generally defined it only in terms of its primary form. This, we argue, reflects a cultural context in which primary control is heavily emphasized and highly valued. In Japan, by contrast, primary control has traditionally been less highly valued and less often anticipated, and secondary control has assumed a more central role in everyday life than in our own culture. To illustrate this cross-cultural difference, we contrast Japanese and American perspectives and practices in child rearing, socialization, religion and philosophy, work, and psychotherapy. These Japanese-American comparisons reveal some key benefits, and some costs, of both primary and secondary approaches to control. In the process, the comparisons reveal the disadvantages of a one-sided pursuit of either form of control. They suggest that an important goal, both for individuals and for cultures, is an optimally adaptive blend of primary and secondary control, a goal best achieved with one's cultural blinders removed. In most American theory and research on the psychology of control, a common theme can be identified; the view that perceived control obtains when individuals shape existing physical, social, or behavioral realities to fit their perceptions, goals, or wishes. According to this view, individuals who do not act to influence such realities may be suffering from learned helplessness (see, e.g., Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978), defects in "self-efficacy" (Bandura, 1977), perceptions of self as a "pawn" (deCharms, 1979), or some form of relinquished control (see, e.g., Langer, 1979). Rothbaum, Weisz, and Snyder (1982) recently spelled out a somewhat broader view. They acknowledged that people do often attempt to gain control by influencing existing realities, often via acts involving personal agency, dominance, or even aggression. Rothbaum et al. labeled this process "primary control." But they argued that control is often sought via alternative paths, which they collectively labeled "secondary control." In secondary control, individuals attempt to align themselves with existing realities, leaving them unchanged but exerting control over their personal psychological impact. Table 1 gives an overview of these two forms of control. Rothbaum et al. reviewed evidence indicating that secondary control often involves behaviors that American investigators have typically classified as signs of relinquished control. For example: 1. Attributing outcomes to low ability combined with behaving in a passive and withdrawn manner, is often labeled helplessness; yet, this combination may often represent an attempt to inhibit unfulfillable expectations, thus preparing oneself for future events and thereby gaining predictive secondary control (e.g., Averill, 1973; Lazarus, 1966; Miller & Grant, 1980), Lefcourt (1973) has reviewed several studies suggesting that prediction allows people to prepare themselves for future events and thus to gain control over the psychological impact of those events. 2. When people attribute outcomes to powerful others and show submissive behavior, they are often thought to have abandoned the pursuit of perceived control; yet, this pattern may foster enhanced identification with the powerful others and thus promote vicarious secondary control (e.g., Hetherington & Frankie, 1967; Johnson & Downing, 1979). Fromm (1941) has written about the human inclination to align oneself with powerful entities (e.g., individuals, groups, or institutions) outside the self in order to enhance one's sense of strength or power. 3. The attribution of outcomes to chance, luck, or fate combined with passivity in or withdrawal from certain competitive skill situations is frequently taken as evidence of relinquished control. However, the combination may often reflect an attempt to be September 1984 • American Psychologist Copyright 1984 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Vol. 39, No. 9, 955-969 955 Table 1 Primary and Secondary Control: An Overview Type of control General strategy Typical targets for causal influence Overall intent

[1]  Nancy Lee Koschmann,et al.  Japanese Culture and Behavior , 1986 .

[2]  E. Skinner,et al.  Control beliefs in an action perspective. , 1984, Human development.

[3]  P. Baltes,et al.  Can I control it? The pursuit of veridical answers across the life-span , 1983 .

[4]  D. Shapiro,et al.  Meta-analysis of comparative therapy outcome studies: a replication and refinement. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  Charles Elliott Managing the Commons , 1982 .

[6]  J. Stigler,et al.  Curriculum and achievement in mathematics: A study of elementary school children in Japan, Taiwan, and the United States. , 1982 .

[7]  R M Dawes,et al.  Psychotherapy outcome: Smith and Glass' conclusions stand up under scrutiny. , 1982 .

[8]  G. Vaillant,et al.  The etiology of alcoholism: a prospective viewpoint. , 1982, The American psychologist.

[9]  E. Zigler,et al.  Learned helplessness and the retarded child , 1982 .

[10]  J. Weisz,et al.  Changing the world and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control. , 1982 .

[11]  H. Evans Internal-External Locus of Control and Word Association , 1981 .

[12]  J. Weisz Learned helplessness in Black and White children identified by their schools as retarded and nonretarded: Performance deterioration in response to failure. , 1981 .

[13]  A. Waterman,et al.  Individualism and interdependence. , 1981 .

[14]  J. Weisz,et al.  Cognitive development in retarded and nonretarded persons: Piagetian tests of the similar structure hypothesis. , 1981, Psychological bulletin.

[15]  D. Reynolds The Quiet Therapies: Japanese Pathways to Personal Growth , 1980 .

[16]  H. Azuma,et al.  Maternal Strategies for Regulating Children's Behavior , 1980 .

[17]  Anthony B. Olejnik Socialization of Achievement , 1980 .

[18]  Lynn R. Kahle,et al.  Stimulus condition self-selection by males in the interaction of locus of control and skill–chance situations. , 1980 .

[19]  Gene V. Glass,et al.  The benefits of psychotherapy , 1980 .

[20]  A. Burgess,et al.  Adaptive strategies and recovery from rape. , 1979, The American journal of psychiatry.

[21]  D. Moody The Good News Bible , 1979 .

[22]  E. Zigler,et al.  Cognitive development in retarded and nonretarded persons: Piagetian tests of the similar sequence hypothesis. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  L. Downing,et al.  Deindividuation and valence of cues: effects on prosocial and antisocial behavior. , 1979, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  J. D. Frank The present status of outcome studies. , 1979, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[25]  F. H. Kanfer Personal Control, Social Control, and Altruism: Can Society Survive the Age of Individualism?. , 1979 .

[26]  Ward J. Fellows Religions East and West , 1979 .

[27]  J. Edney,et al.  The commons dilemma: A simulation testing the effects of resource visibility and territorial division , 1978 .

[28]  J. Edney,et al.  The commons dilemma , 1978 .

[29]  M. Toda,et al.  Development of competitive behavior as a function of culture, age, and social comparison. , 1978 .

[30]  Douglas S. Noonan,et al.  Managing the Commons , 1978 .

[31]  H. Eysenck An exercise in mega-silliness. , 1978 .

[32]  M. Seligman,et al.  Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. , 1978, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[33]  M. L. Smith,et al.  Meta-analysis of psychotherapy outcome studies. , 1977, The American psychologist.

[34]  C. Wortman,et al.  Attributions of blame and coping in the "real world": severe accident victims react to their lot. , 1977, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  A. Bandura Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. , 1977, Psychological review.

[36]  R. Ringer Looking out for number one , 1977 .

[37]  D. Meichenbaum Cognitive-Behavior Modification , 1977 .

[38]  D. Campbell On the conflicts between biological and social evolution and between psychology and moral tradition. , 1976, Zygon.

[39]  R. Lazarus Patterns of Adjustment , 1976 .

[40]  George A. De Vos,et al.  Socialization for Achievement. , 1976 .

[41]  T. Lebra,et al.  Japanese patterns of behavior , 1976 .

[42]  R. Lazarus Patterns of adjustment, 3rd ed. , 1976 .

[43]  Michael Korda,et al.  Power!: How to Get It, How to Use It , 1975 .

[44]  R. Hogan Theoretical egocentrism and the problem of compliance. , 1975 .

[45]  今井 正明 Never take yes for an answer : an inside look at Japanese business , 1975 .

[46]  F. Hsu Iemoto: the heart of Japan , 1975 .

[47]  D. Mcclelland Power: The Inner Experience , 1975 .

[48]  S. Milgram Obedience to Authority: An Experimental View , 1975 .

[49]  I. Mahler A comparative study of locus of control. , 1974 .

[50]  O. Parsons,et al.  Locus of control in university students from Eastern and Western societies. , 1974 .

[51]  F. Fürstenberg Why the Japanese have been so successful in business , 1974 .

[52]  E. McGinnies,et al.  Sex and Cultural Differences in Perceived Locus of Control Among Students in Five Countries. , 1974 .

[53]  S. Wolk,et al.  Cognitive and motivational correlates of generalized expectancies for control. , 1973, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[54]  H. Lefcourt The function of the illusions of control and freedom. , 1973, The American psychologist.

[55]  A. M. Whitehill,et al.  The Other Worker: A Comparative Study of Industrial Relations in the United States and Japan. , 1969, Modern Asian Studies.

[56]  Robert E. Cole,et al.  British factory, Japanese factory , 1973 .

[57]  J. Averill Personal control over aversive stimuli and its relationship to stress. , 1973 .

[58]  別府 春海,et al.  Japan : an anthropological introduction , 1971 .

[59]  M. Miura,et al.  A psychotherapy of neurosis, Morita therapy. , 1970, Yonago acta medica.

[60]  B. Crowe The tragedy of the commons revisite. , 1969, Science.

[61]  J. Werry,et al.  The Token Economy: A Motivational System for Therapy and Rehabilitation. , 1969 .

[62]  W Caudill,et al.  Maternal care and infant behavior in Japan and America. , 1969, Psychiatry.

[63]  T. Merton Faith and Violence: Christian Teaching and Christian Practice , 1968 .

[64]  H. Eysenck Behavior Therapy Techniques , 1968 .

[65]  E. M. Hetherington,et al.  Effects of parental dominance, warmth, and conflict on imitation in children. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[66]  R. Lazarus Psychological stress and the coping process , 1970 .

[67]  D. Plath,et al.  Who Sleeps by Whom? Parent-Child Involvement in Urban Japanese Families †. , 1966, Psychiatry.

[68]  W. H. Brown Japanese Management. The Cultural Background , 1966 .

[69]  J. Rotter,et al.  Internal versus external control of reinforcement and decision time. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[70]  T. Merton Contemplation in a world of action , 1965 .

[71]  Eli Thomas Reich,et al.  American Society , 2004, Beautiful Imperialist.

[72]  Arthur M Whitehall Cultural values and employee attitudes: United States and Japan. , 1964 .

[73]  L. T. Doi AMAE - A KEY CONCEPT FOR UNDERSTANDING JAPANESE PERSONALITY STRUCTURE , 1962, PSYCHOLOGIA.

[74]  E. Vogel,et al.  Family Security, Personal Immaturity, and Emotional Health in a Japanese Sample , 1961 .

[75]  B. Spock Baby and Child Care , 1959 .

[76]  Ronald Dore,et al.  City Life in Japan , 1959 .

[77]  H. Hartmann Ego psychology and the problem of adaptation , 1958 .

[78]  C. Morris Varieties of Human Value , 1957 .

[79]  S. Asch Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. , 1956 .

[80]  L. V. Jones,et al.  Value scales and dimensions. , 1955, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[81]  Lewis Mumford,et al.  The human prospect , 1955 .

[82]  R. Robison Morita Therapy: A Japanese Therapy for Neurosis , 1954 .

[83]  P. Lawrence The Chrysanthemum and the Sword , 1948, Nature.

[84]  Sigmund Freud,et al.  New Introductory Lectures on Psycho-Analysis , 1933 .