Integration and segregation of multiple motion signals by neurons in area MT of primate.

We used multielectrode arrays to measure the response of populations of neurons in primate middle temporal area to the transparent motion of two superimposed dot fields moving in different directions. The shape of the population response was well predicted by the sum of the responses to the constituent fields. However, the population response profile for transparent dot fields was similar to that for coherent plaid motion and hence an unreliable cue to transparency. We then used single-unit recording to characterize component and pattern cells from their response to drifting plaids. Unlike for plaids, component cells responded to the average direction of superimposed dot fields, whereas pattern cells could signal the constituent motions. This observation provides support for a strong prediction of the Simoncelli and Heeger (1998) model of motion analysis in area middle temporal, and suggests that pattern cells have a special status in the processing of superimposed dot fields.

[1]  P. Lennie,et al.  Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivities of neurones in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  Charles G. Gross,et al.  Pattern recognition mechanisms , 1985 .

[3]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  Thomas D. Albright,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual motion coherence , 1992, Nature.

[5]  Roger B. H. Tootell,et al.  Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1992, Nature.

[6]  A. Rosenfeld,et al.  Perceptual motion transparency : the role of geometrical information , 1992 .

[7]  R. Born,et al.  Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1993, Nature.

[8]  H R Wilson,et al.  A model for motion coherence and transparency , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.

[9]  Peter E. Latham,et al.  Statistically Efficient Estimation Using Population Coding , 1998, Neural Computation.

[10]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  A model of neuronal responses in visual area MT , 1998, Vision Research.

[11]  W. Newsome,et al.  The Variable Discharge of Cortical Neurons: Implications for Connectivity, Computation, and Information Coding , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  G. Elston,et al.  Visuotopic organisation and neuronal response selectivity for direction of motion in visual areas of the caudal temporal lobe of the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus): Middle temporal area, middle temporal crescent, and surrounding cortex , 1998, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[13]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Probabilistic Interpretation of Population Codes , 1996, Neural Computation.

[14]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Motion transparency: making models of motion perception transparent , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  R. Zemel,et al.  Information processing with population codes , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[16]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Neural synchrony correlates with surface segregation rules , 2000, Nature.

[17]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Seeing multiple directions of motion—physiology and psychophysics , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Theoretical Neuroscience: Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural Systems , 2001 .

[19]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Activity patterns in human motion sensitive areas depend on the interpretation of global motion , 2001, NeuroImage.

[20]  Thorsten Joachims,et al.  Learning to classify text using support vector machines - methods, theory and algorithms , 2002, The Kluwer international series in engineering and computer science.

[21]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Simultaneous encoding of multiple potential reach directions in dorsal premotor cortex. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  William Curran,et al.  Directional performance in motion transparency , 2002, Vision Research.

[23]  A. Thiele,et al.  Neuronal synchrony does not correlate with motion coherence in cortical area MT , 2003, Nature.

[24]  Nava Rubin,et al.  The dynamics of bi-stable alternation in ambiguous motion displays: a fresh look at plaids , 2003, Vision Research.

[25]  Margaret S Livingstone,et al.  End-Stopping and the Aperture Problem Two-Dimensional Motion Signals in Macaque V1 , 2003, Neuron.

[26]  J. Movshon,et al.  Dynamics of motion signaling by neurons in macaque area MT , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  Ralf Wessel,et al.  Motion repulsion arises from stimulus statistics when analyzed with a clustering algorithm , 2005, Biological Cybernetics.

[28]  P. Latham,et al.  Synergy, Redundancy, and Independence in Population Codes, Revisited , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  How MT cells analyze the motion of visual patterns , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  A. Pouget,et al.  Neural correlations, population coding and computation , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[31]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  The role of V1 surround suppression in MT motion integration. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Chris Tailby,et al.  Visual motion integration by neurons in the middle temporal area of a New World monkey, the marmoset , 2011, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  Arnulf B. A. Graf,et al.  Decoding the activity of neuronal populations in macaque primary visual cortex , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  J. Gallant,et al.  A Three-Dimensional Spatiotemporal Receptive Field Model Explains Responses of Area MT Neurons to Naturalistic Movies , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.