A theory of actions and habits: The interaction of rate correlation and contiguity systems in free-operant behavior.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C. L. Hull. Principles of behavior : an introduction to behavior theory , 1943 .
[2] W. Estes. Discriminative conditioning; effects of a Pavlovian conditioned stimulus upon a subsequently established operant response. , 1948, Journal of experimental psychology.
[3] R. R. Bush,et al. A Mathematical Model for Simple Learning , 1951 .
[4] B. Underwood,et al. ASSOCIATION BY CONTIGUITY. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.
[5] R. Rescorla. Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. , 1967, Psychological review.
[6] J. Konorski. Integrative activity of the brain : an interdisciplinary approach , 1967 .
[7] R. Weisman,et al. Positive conditioned reinforcement of Sidman avoidance behavior in rats. , 1969 .
[8] R. Herrnstein. Method and theory in the study of avoidance. , 1969, Psychological review.
[9] R. Rescorla,et al. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .
[10] J. C. Johnston,et al. A cognitive theory of avoidance learning. , 1973 .
[11] W M Baum,et al. The correlation-based law of effect. , 1973, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[12] G. Morrison,et al. Taste-mediated conditioned aversion to an exteroceptive stimulus following LiCl poisoning. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[13] N. Mackintosh. The psychology of animal learning , 1974 .
[14] E. Holman. Some conditions for the dissociation of consummatory and instrumental behavior in rats , 1975 .
[15] J. Platt,et al. Reinforcement rate and interresponse time differentiation. , 1976, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[16] R. Herrnstein,et al. Toward a law of response strength. , 1976 .
[17] T. J. Matthews,et al. Yoked variable-ratio and variable-interval responding in pigeons. , 1977, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[18] S. Hanson,et al. Arousal: its genesis and manifestation as response rate. , 1978, Psychological review.
[19] Gene M. Heyman,et al. Matching and Maximizing in Concurrent Schedules , 1979 .
[20] L. J. Hammond. The effect of contingency upon the appetitive conditioning of free-operant behavior. , 1980, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[21] Christopher D. Adams. Post-Conditioning Devaluation of an Instrumental Reinforcer has no Effect on Extinction Performance , 1980 .
[22] Christopher D. Adams,et al. Instrumental Responding following Reinforcer Devaluation , 1981 .
[23] A. Dickinson. Contemporary Animal Learning Theory , 1981 .
[24] C. Bradshaw,et al. Relationship between response rate and reinforcement frequency in variable-interval schedules: III. The effect of d-amphetamine. , 1981, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[25] Drazen Prelec,et al. Matching, maximizing, and the hyperbolic reinforcement feedback function. , 1982 .
[26] Christopher D. Adams. Variations in the Sensitivity of Instrumental Responding to Reinforcer Devaluation , 1982 .
[27] P R Killeen,et al. Incentive theory: II. Models for choice. , 1982, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[28] Christopher D. Adams,et al. The Effect of the Instrumental Training Contingency on Susceptibility to Reinforcer Devaluation , 1983 .
[29] J. E. Mazur,et al. Steady-state performance on fixed-, mixed-, and random-ratio schedules. , 1983, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[30] A. Silberberg,et al. Primacy of interresponse-time reinforcement in accounting for rate differences under variable-ratio and variable-interval schedules. , 1984 .
[31] A. Dickinson. Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy , 1985 .
[32] R. Rescorla,et al. Instrumental responding remains sensitive to reinforcer devaluation after extensive training , 1985 .
[33] A. Dickinson,et al. Pavlovian Processes in the Motivational Control of Instrumental Performance , 1987 .
[34] A. Dickinson,et al. Motivational Control of Instrumental Performance: The Role of Prior Experience of The Reinforcer , 1988 .
[35] R. Rescorla,et al. Associations between the discriminative stimulus and the reinforcer in instrumental learning. , 1988 .
[36] J. Wearden,et al. Interresponse-time reinforcement and behavior under aperiodic reinforcement schedules: A case study using computer modeling. , 1988 .
[37] A. Dickinson,et al. Incentive Learning and the Motivational Control of Instrumental Performance , 1989 .
[38] A. Dickinson,et al. The Intentionality of Animal Action , 1990 .
[39] R. Rescorla,et al. Evidence for the hierarchical structure of instrumental learning , 1990 .
[40] A. Dickinson,et al. Performance on Ratio and Interval Schedules with Matched Reinforcement Rates , 1990, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[41] R. Rescorla. Evidence for an association between the discriminative stimulus and the response-outcome association in instrumental learning. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[42] A. Dickinson,et al. Instrumental judgment and performance under variations in action-outcome contingency and contiguity , 1991, Memory & cognition.
[43] R. Colwill. Negative discriminative stimuli provide information about the identity of omitted response-contingent outcomes , 1991 .
[44] W. J. Griffiths,et al. Free-Operant Acquisition with Delayed Reinforcement , 1992 .
[45] B. Balleine. Instrumental performance following a shift in primary motivation depends on incentive learning. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[46] R. Rescorla. Response-independent outcome presentation can leave instrumental R-O associations intact , 1992 .
[47] W M Baum,et al. In search of the feedback function for variable-interval schedules. , 1992, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[48] R. Rescorla. Preservation of response-outcome associations through extinction , 1993 .
[49] B. Balleine,et al. Motivational control of goal-directed action , 1994 .
[50] P. Killeen. Mathematical principles of reinforcement , 1994 .
[51] R. Rescorla. Transfer of instrumental control mediated by a devalued outcome , 1994 .
[52] M. Bouton,et al. Renewal of extinguished responding in a second context , 1994 .
[53] R. Boakes,et al. Motivational control after extended instrumental training , 1995 .
[54] B. Balleine,et al. Bidirectional Instrumental Conditioning , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[55] A. Dickinson,et al. Omission Learning after Instrumental Pretraining , 1998 .
[56] B. Balleine,et al. Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates , 1998, Neuropharmacology.
[57] Phil Reed. Schedules of reinforcement as determinants of human causality judgments and response rates. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[58] S. Killcross,et al. Inactivation of the infralimbic prefrontal cortex reinstates goal-directed responding in overtrained rats , 2003, Behavioural Brain Research.
[59] S. Killcross,et al. Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[60] P. Holland. Relations between Pavlovian-instrumental transfer and reinforcer devaluation. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[61] Edgar H Vogel,et al. Quantitative models of Pavlovian conditioning , 2004, Brain Research Bulletin.
[62] P. Dayan,et al. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[63] Richard S. Sutton,et al. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.
[64] Peter Dayan,et al. How fast to work: Response vigor, motivation and tonic dopamine , 2005, NIPS.
[65] S. Killcross,et al. Amphetamine Exposure Enhances Habit Formation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[66] P. Dayan,et al. Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor , 2007, Psychopharmacology.
[67] J. J. McDowell,et al. Feedback functions, optimization, and the relation of response rate to reinforcer rate. , 2006, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[68] P. Reed. Human sensitivity to reinforcement feedback functions , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[69] Vivian V. Valentin,et al. Determining the Neural Substrates of Goal-Directed Learning in the Human Brain , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[70] Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al. Learning the value of information in an uncertain world , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[71] On the primacy of molecular processes in determining response rates under variable-ratio and variable-interval schedules. , 2008, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[72] B. Balleine,et al. A specific role for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[73] T. Maia. Reinforcement learning, conditioning, and the brain: Successes and challenges , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[74] E. Thorndike. Animal Intelligence; Experimental Studies , 2009 .
[75] R. Costa,et al. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience Integrative Neuroscience , 2022 .
[76] Fabian A. Soto,et al. Error-driven learning in visual categorization and object recognition: a common-elements model. , 2010, Psychological review.
[77] B. Balleine,et al. Human and Rodent Homologies in Action Control: Corticostriatal Determinants of Goal-Directed and Habitual Action , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.
[78] E. Coutureau,et al. Pavlovian to instrumental transfer: A neurobehavioural perspective , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[79] A. Dickinson,et al. Choice and contingency in the development of behavioral autonomy during instrumental conditioning. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[80] A. Dickinson,et al. The role of contextual conditioning in the effect of reinforcer devaluation on instrumental performance by rats , 2010, Behavioural Processes.
[81] P. Dayan,et al. Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors , 2011, Neuron.
[82] Amir Dezfouli,et al. Speed/Accuracy Trade-Off between the Habitual and the Goal-Directed Processes , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[83] Justin A. Harris,et al. Response rate and reinforcement rate in Pavlovian conditioning. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[84] T. Robbins,et al. Disruption in the Balance Between Goal-Directed Behavior and Habit Learning in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder , 2011, The American journal of psychiatry.
[85] N. Daw,et al. The ubiquity of model-based reinforcement learning , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[86] A. Dickinson. Associative learning and animal cognition , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[87] A. Silberberg,et al. The copyist model of response emission , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[88] Xin Jin,et al. Different dorsal striatum circuits mediate action discrimination and action generalization , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.
[89] Alec Solway,et al. Goal-directed decision making as probabilistic inference: a computational framework and potential neural correlates. , 2012, Psychological review.
[90] Brian J. Wiltgen,et al. The Effect of Ratio and Interval Training on Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer in Mice , 2012, PloS one.
[91] B. Balleine,et al. Habits, action sequences and reinforcement learning , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.
[92] Bernard W. Balleine,et al. Actions, Action Sequences and Habits: Evidence That Goal-Directed and Habitual Action Control Are Hierarchically Organized , 2013, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[93] R. Costa,et al. Orbitofrontal and striatal circuits dynamically encode the shift between goal-directed and habitual actions , 2013, Nature Communications.
[94] F. Cushman. Action, Outcome, and Value , 2013, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
[95] E. Murray,et al. Differential Effects of Amygdala, Orbital Prefrontal Cortex, and Prelimbic Cortex Lesions on Goal-Directed Behavior in Rhesus Macaques , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[96] P. Dayan,et al. Goals and Habits in the Brain , 2013, Neuron.
[97] B. Balleine,et al. Effects of Repeated Cocaine Exposure on Habit Learning and Reversal by N-Acetylcysteine , 2014, Neuropsychopharmacology.
[98] A. Dickinson,et al. Safety signals as instrumental reinforcers during free-operant avoidance , 2014, Learning & memory.
[99] Fabian A. Soto,et al. Explaining compound generalization in associative and causal learning through rational principles of dimensional generalization. , 2014, Psychological review.
[100] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Neural Computations Underlying Arbitration between Model-Based and Model-free Learning , 2013, Neuron.
[101] A. R. Wagner. SOP: A Model of Automatic Memory Processing in Animal Behavior , 2014 .
[102] A. Dickinson,et al. Free-Operant Avoidance Behavior by Rats after Reinforcer Revaluation Using Opioid Agonists and d-Amphetamine , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[103] N. Daw,et al. Model-based learning protects against forming habits , 2015, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.
[104] M. Bouton,et al. Contextual control of instrumental actions and habits. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[105] Samuel J Gershman,et al. Do learning rates adapt to the distribution of rewards? , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[106] T. Robbins,et al. An Associative Account of Avoidance , 2016 .
[107] Wouter Kool,et al. When Does Model-Based Control Pay Off? , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[108] B. Balleine,et al. Appetitive Pavlovian-instrumental Transfer: A review , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[109] Wouter Kool,et al. Cost-Benefit Arbitration Between Multiple Reinforcement-Learning Systems , 2017, Psychological science.
[110] Stefano Palminteri,et al. Confirmation bias in human reinforcement learning: Evidence from counterfactual feedback processing , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[111] A. Dickinson,et al. Actions and Habits: Psychological Issues in Dual-System Theory , 2018 .
[112] A. Dickinson,et al. A re-examination of responding on ratio and regulated-probability interval schedules , 2018, bioRxiv.
[113] Sarah L. Knot,et al. Shifting the Balance Between Goals and Habits: Five Failures in Experimental Habit Induction , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[114] Goal-directed control on interval schedules does not depend on the action-outcome correlation. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[115] Fabian A. Soto,et al. Human instrumental performance in ratio and interval contingencies: A challenge for associative theory , 2019, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[116] B. Balleine,et al. Hierarchical Action Control: Adaptive Collaboration Between Actions and Habits , 2019, Front. Psychol..
[117] Bernard W. Balleine,et al. The Meaning of Behavior: Discriminating Reflex and Volition in the Brain , 2019, Neuron.
[118] Andrew T. Marshall,et al. Mesolimbic dopamine projections mediate cue-motivated reward seeking but not reward retrieval in rats , 2018, eLife.
[119] Kevin J. Miller,et al. Habits without Values , 2016, bioRxiv.
[120] G. Urcelay,et al. Delayed rewards facilitate habit formation. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[121] Fabian A. Soto,et al. Evidence for a dissociation between causal beliefs and instrumental actions , 2018, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[122] M. Bouton,et al. Unexpected food outcomes can return a habit to goal-directed action , 2020, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[123] M. Bouton,et al. Some factors that restore goal-direction to a habitual behavior , 2020, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.