Acquisition of Landmark Knowledge from Static and Dynamic Presentation of Route Maps Paul U. Lee (pauly@psych.stanford.edu) Department of Psychology, Bldg. 420 Stanford, CA 94305 USA Heike Tappe & Alexander Klippel ([tappe, klippel]@informatik.uni-hamburg.de) Department for Informatics, University of Hamburg Vogt-Kolln-Str. 30, 22527 Hamburg, Germany Introduction Route maps have recently gained much attention as effective tools to convey route information. Benefits of maps are attributed to their ubiquitous existence in culture and their analogous properties representing spatial knowledge. Route maps have become widely available through the internet and within on-board navigation systems. Despite their prevalence, optimal design criteria are still missing. For example, route maps integrated in on-board navigation systems present routes dynamically with a moving dot that traverses a map. In contrast, internet maps present information statically with lines representing the route. At first glance, dynamic, animated presentation seems to be more effective than static one. (e.g. Nathan, Kintsch, & Young, 1992). However, advantages of animation may be due to other factors, such as interactivity or inclusion of information not present in static conditions. Furthermore, other studies fail to demonstrate superiority of animations at all (e.g. Morrison, 2000). Extending research on effects of static vs. dynamic route presentation on conceptualization and memory (Klippel, Tappe, Habel, submitted), we examined the influence of presentation mode on memory for landmarks. Dynamic vs. Static Presentation of Maps Material and Procedure Participants learned a route from a map of a fictitious town. The route was presented to them either as a solid line (i.e. static), a moving dot (dynamic), or a dot superimposed on a line (mixed). The participants viewed the map three times, each for 1.5 minutes. Afterwards, they were given a blank map with only the streets and were asked to recall the landmarks. Recall Memory of Landmarks In the dynamic condition, landmarks at turning and non- turning intersections were recalled equally well (49.4% vs. 48.8%), but in the static condition landmarks at turning intersections were remembered more often (52.9%) than at non-turning intersections (43.8%) (see Table 1). Since landmarks at turns are more critical to route directions, we conclude that static displays of route information is preferable over dynamic displays. Dynamic Static Mixed Turns Non-turns Total Table 1: Proportion of recalled landmarks (in %) Surprisingly, in the mixed condition participants recalled even more landmarks at turns (57.7%) than at non-turns (41.5%). The combination of different presentation modes and the resulting memory improvement for vital route information support findings on the benefits of redundant information displays (Hirtle, 1999). Acknowledgments This research was supported by DAAD PKZ A-01-49336 to the first author and by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) HA (Conceptualization processes in language production), and FR, 806-8 (Aspect maps) to the second and the third author. References Hirtle, S. C. (1999). The use of maps, images, and “gestures” for navigation (pp. 31-40). In C. Freksa, W. Brauer, C. Habel, K.F. Wender (eds.). Spatial cognition II, integrating abstract theories, empirical studies, formal methods, and practical applications. Springer: Berlin. Klippel, A., Tappe, H., & Habel, C. (submitted). Pictorial representation of routes. Chunking route segments during comprehension. Morrison, J. B. (2000). Does animation facilitate learning? An evaluation of the congruence and equivalence hypothesis. Ph.D. Thesis, Stanford University. Nathan, M. J., Kintsch, W., & Young, E. (1992). A theory of algebra-word-problem comprehension and its implications for the design of learning environments. Cognition and Instruction, 9, 329-389.
[1]
N. Hari Narayanan,et al.
Multimedia instruction: lessons from evaluation of a theory-based design
,
1999
.
[2]
Alexander Klippel,et al.
Pictorial Representations of Routes: Chunking Route Segments during Comprehension
,
2003,
Spatial Cognition.
[3]
W. Klein.
Local deixis in route directions
,
1982
.
[4]
Stephen C. Hirtle.
The Use of Maps, Images and "Gestures" for Navigation
,
2000,
Spatial Cognition.
[5]
Chris Stolte,et al.
Rendering effective route maps: improving usability through generalization
,
2001,
SIGGRAPH.
[6]
Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.
Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events.
,
2001,
Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[7]
H. Couclelis.
VERBAL DIRECTIONS FOR WAY-FINDING: SPACE, COGNITION, AND LANGUAGE
,
1996
.
[8]
M. Denis.
The description of routes : A cognitive approach to the production of spatial discourse
,
1997
.
[9]
Paul U. Lee,et al.
How Space Structures Language
,
1998,
Spatial Cognition.
[10]
David E. Kieras,et al.
Diagrammatic Displays for Engineered Systems: Effects on Human Performance in Interacting with Malfunctioning Systems
,
1992,
Int. J. Man Mach. Stud..
[11]
Mitchell J. Nathan,et al.
A theory of algebra-word-problem comprehension and its implications for the design of learning environments.
,
1992
.
[12]
J. Portugali.
The construction of cognitive maps
,
1996
.
[13]
Julie Bauer Morrison,et al.
Does animation facilitate learning? An evaluation of the congruence and equivalence hypotheses
,
2000
.
[14]
P G Jackson,et al.
In search of better route guidance instructions.
,
1998,
Ergonomics.