Classification of Passive Auditory Event-Related Potentials Using Discriminant Analysis and Self-Organizing Feature Maps

Discriminant analysis (DA) and self-organizing feature maps (SOFM) were used to classify passively evoked auditory event-related potentials (ERP) P1, N1, P2 and N2. Responses from 16 children with severe behavioral auditory perception deficits, 16 children with marked behavioral auditory perception deficits, and 14 controls were examined. Eighteen ERP amplitude parameters were selected for examination of statistical differences between the groups. Different DA methods and SOFM configurations were trained to the values. SOFM had better classification results than DA methods. Subsequently, measures on another 37 subjects that were unknown for the trained SOFM were used to test the reliability of the system. With 10-dimensional vectors, reliable classifications were obtained that matched behavioral auditory perception deficits in 96%, implying central auditory processing disorder (CAPD). The results also support the assumption that CAPD includes a ‘non-peripheral’ auditory processing deficit.

[1]  Jörg Kopecz,et al.  Neuronale Netze: Grundlagen und Anwendungen , 1995 .

[2]  Nina Kraus,et al.  Absent auditory brain stem response: Peripheral hearing loss or brain stem dysfunction? , 1984, The Laryngoscope.

[3]  T. Yamada,et al.  AUDITORY EVENT‐RELATED POTENTIALS AND REACTION TIME IN CHILDREN EVALUATION OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT , 1993, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[4]  M. Studdert-Kennedy,et al.  Auditory temporal perception deficits in the reading-impaired: A critical review of the evidence , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  E. Schröger The influence of stimulus intensity and inter-stimulus interval on the detection of pitch and loudness changes. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[6]  M M Merzenich,et al.  Neural Mechanisms Underlying Temporal Integration, Segmentation, and Input Sequence Representation: Some Implications for the Origin of Learning Disabilities a , 1993, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[7]  T. Järvilehto,et al.  AUDITORY and VISUAL EVOKED POTENTIALS OF SCHOOLBOYS WITH SPELLING DISABILITIES , 1985, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[8]  Jorma Laaksonen,et al.  SOM_PAK: The Self-Organizing Map Program Package , 1996 .

[9]  Paula Tallal,et al.  Identification of language-impaired children on the basis of rapid perception and production skills , 1985, Brain and Language.

[10]  E Gordon,et al.  The effects of age on auditory event-related potentials. , 1991, Experimental aging research.

[11]  Linda J. Lombardino,et al.  Deficits in auditory temporal and spectral resolution in language-impaired children , 1997, Nature.

[12]  F. Bloom,et al.  P300 from an auditory oddball task: inter-laboratory consistency. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[13]  Can Children with Language Impairment Be Accurately Identified Using Temporal Processing Measures? A Simulation Study , 1998, Brain and Language.

[14]  H Begleiter,et al.  Hemispheric differences for P300 amplitude from an auditory oddball task. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[15]  H. Ritter,et al.  Self-organizing maps for internal representations , 1990, Psychological research.

[16]  John Polich,et al.  Cognitive Brain Potentials , 1993 .

[17]  D. Watson,et al.  A NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL STUDY OF CHILDREN WITH READING, WRITING AND SPELLING DIFFICULTIES , 1989, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[18]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  Multichannel auditory event-related brain potentials: effects of normal aging on the scalp distribution of N1, P2, N2 and P300 latencies and amplitudes. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[19]  H Lyytinen,et al.  Auditory event-related potentials in the study of developmental language-related disorders. , 1997, Audiology & neuro-otology.

[20]  E Schröger,et al.  The influence of stimulus intensity and inter-stimulus interval on the detection of pitch and loudness changes , 1996 .

[21]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 from a single auditory stimulus. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[22]  F. Perrin,et al.  Brain generators implicated in the processing of auditory stimulus deviance: a topographic event-related potential study. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[23]  M. Hyde The N1 response and its applications. , 1997, Audiology & neuro-otology.

[24]  M. Schröer,et al.  Auditive Wahrnehmungsstörungen und Fehlhörigkeit bei Kindern im Schulalter , 1987 .

[25]  H. Persson,et al.  Topography of auditory evoked cortical potentials in children with severe language impairment: the N1 component. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[26]  A J Gabor,et al.  Automated seizure detection using a self-organizing neural network. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[27]  Teuvo Kohonen,et al.  The self-organizing map , 1990, Neurocomputing.

[28]  Carl G. Looney,et al.  Pattern recognition using neural networks: theory and algorithms for engineers and scientists , 1997 .

[29]  F. Mauguière,et al.  Revisiting the oddball paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional effects , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  Jack S. Damico,et al.  Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: The Scope of the Problem , 1995, Seminars in speech and language.

[31]  T Kohonen,et al.  Associative memory and pattern recognition. , 1978, Medical biology.

[32]  M Kajola,et al.  Neural net identification of thumb movement using spectral characteristics of magnetic cortical rhythms. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[33]  R. Näätänen Implications of ERP data for psychological theories of attention , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[34]  J. Pernier,et al.  Two separate frontal components in the N1 wave of the human auditory evoked response. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[35]  T. Carrell,et al.  The mismatch negativity cortical evoked potential elicited by speech in cochlear-implant users , 1993, Hearing Research.

[36]  T. Carrell,et al.  Mismatch Negativity in the Neurophysiologic/Behavioral Evaluation of Auditory Processing Deficits: A Case Study , 1993, Ear and hearing.

[37]  K B Campbell,et al.  The effect of sleep onset on event related potentials with rapid rates of stimulus presentation. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[38]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Mismatch negativity--the measure for central sound representation accuracy. , 1997, Audiology & neuro-otology.

[39]  W. Baxt Application of artificial neural networks to clinical medicine , 1995, The Lancet.

[40]  J. Polich,et al.  Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[41]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses , 1997, Nature.

[42]  Jorma Laaksonen,et al.  LVQ_PAK: The Learning Vector Quantization Program Package , 1996 .

[43]  T. Carrell,et al.  Auditory Neurophysiologic Responses and Discrimination Deficits in Children with Learning Problems , 1996, Science.

[44]  Paula Tallal,et al.  The relationship between auditory temporal analysis and receptive language development: Evidence from studies of developmental language disorder , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  Preattentive periodicity detection in auditory patterns as governed by time and intensity information. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[46]  John Polich,et al.  P300 from auditory stimuli: intensity and frequency effects , 1995, Biological Psychology.