A Bayesian account of ‘hysteria’

This article provides a neurobiological account of symptoms that have been called ‘hysterical’, ‘psychogenic’ or ‘medically unexplained’, which we will call functional motor and sensory symptoms. We use a neurobiologically informed model of hierarchical Bayesian inference in the brain to explain functional motor and sensory symptoms in terms of perception and action arising from inference based on prior beliefs and sensory information. This explanation exploits the key balance between prior beliefs and sensory evidence that is mediated by (body focused) attention, symptom expectations, physical and emotional experiences and beliefs about illness. Crucially, this furnishes an explanation at three different levels: (i) underlying neuromodulatory (synaptic) mechanisms; (ii) cognitive and experiential processes (attention and attribution of agency); and (iii) formal computations that underlie perceptual inference (representation of uncertainty or precision). Our explanation involves primary and secondary failures of inference; the primary failure is the (autonomous) emergence of a percept or belief that is held with undue certainty (precision) following top-down attentional modulation of synaptic gain. This belief can constitute a sensory percept (or its absence) or induce movement (or its absence). The secondary failure of inference is when the ensuing percept (and any somatosensory consequences) is falsely inferred to be a symptom to explain why its content was not predicted by the source of attentional modulation. This account accommodates several fundamental observations about functional motor and sensory symptoms, including: (i) their induction and maintenance by attention; (ii) their modification by expectation, prior experience and cultural beliefs and (iii) their involuntary and symptomatic nature.

[1]  J. Reynolds Remarks on Paralysis, and other Disorders of Motion and Sensation, Dependent on Idea* , 1869, British medical journal.

[2]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[3]  P. Janet The Major Symptoms of Hysteria , 1908, Bristol Medico-Chirurgical Journal (1883).

[4]  A. Ludwig,et al.  Hysteria. A neurobiological theory. , 1972, Archives of general psychiatry.

[5]  R. Lévy,et al.  The somatosensory evoked response in patients with hysterical anaesthesia. , 1973, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[6]  R. Davidson,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory IV , 1976 .

[7]  E. Hilgard Divided Consciousness: Multiple Controls In Human Thought And Action , 1977 .

[8]  J. Winn,et al.  Brain , 1878, The Lancet.

[9]  William C. Ogden,et al.  Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location , 2014 .

[10]  J. Springett,et al.  Diagnosis of hysteria. , 1980, The New Zealand medical journal.

[11]  D. Wegner,et al.  Arousal-induced attention to self , 1980 .

[12]  J. Friedman,et al.  Attention in hysteria: a study of Janet's hypothesis by means of habituation and arousal measures. , 1980, The American journal of psychiatry.

[13]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[14]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  Competition of internal and external information in an exercise setting. , 1980, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  Selective monitoring of physical sensations. , 1981, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  James W. Pennebaker,et al.  The psychology of physical symptoms , 1982 .

[17]  S. Guze Studies in Hysteria * , 1983, Canadian journal of psychiatry. Revue canadienne de psychiatrie.

[18]  D. LaBerge Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  B. Libet,et al.  Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  M. Hersen,et al.  Adult psychopathology and diagnosis , 1984 .

[21]  R. Fillingim,et al.  The effects of internal versus external information processing on symptom perception in an exercise setting. , 1986, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[22]  D. Mechanic,et al.  The Socialization of Introspection and Illness Behaviour , 1986 .

[23]  S. Mchugh,et al.  Illness Behavior: A Multidisciplinary Model , 1987 .

[24]  O. Vassend Dimensions of negative affectivity, self-reported somatic symptoms, and health-related behaviors. , 1989, Social science & medicine.

[25]  D. Stewart,et al.  The changing faces of somatization. , 1990, Psychosomatics.

[26]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Preface: Cerebral Cortex Has Come of Age , 1991 .

[27]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  T. Bayes An essay towards solving a problem in the doctrine of chances , 2003 .

[29]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  S. Benjamin,et al.  Abnormal illness behaviour: childhood experiences and long-term consequences , 1992 .

[31]  D Mumford,et al.  On the computational architecture of the neocortex. II. The role of cortico-cortical loops. , 1992, Biological cybernetics.

[32]  J. Kihlstrom Hypnosis: a sesquicentennial essay. , 1992, International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis.

[33]  A. Wing,et al.  Coordination of aimed movements in a case of unilateral cerebellar damage , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Psychological mechanisms in hypochondriasis: attention-induced physical symptoms without sensory stimulation. , 1994, Psychotherapy and psychosomatics.

[35]  P. Spinhoven,et al.  The Development and Psychometric Characteristics of the Somatoform Dissociation Questionnaire (SDQ‐20) , 1996, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[36]  P. Andersen,et al.  Clinical characteristics of patients with motor disability due to conversion disorder: a prospective control group study , 1997, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[37]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Anatomy of motor learning. I. Frontal cortex and attention to action. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  D. Brooks,et al.  A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Gereon R Fink,et al.  The functional anatomy of a hysterical paralysis , 1997, Cognition.

[40]  C. Marsden,et al.  Slater revisited: 6 year follow up study of patients with medically unexplained motor symptoms , 1998, BMJ.

[41]  R. Djaldetti,et al.  Diagnosis of “non-organic” limb paresis by a novel objective motor assessment: the quantitative Hoover’s test , 1998, Journal of Neurology.

[42]  Frank Baeyens,et al.  When somatic information threatens, catastrophic thinking enhances attentional interference , 1998, Pain.

[43]  R. Mayou Chronic fatigue and its syndromes , 1999, BMJ.

[44]  P. Garety,et al.  Cognitive approaches to delusions: a critical review of theories and evidence. , 1999, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[45]  M. Hotopf,et al.  Childhood risk factors for adults with medically unexplained symptoms: results from a national birth cohort study. , 1999, The American journal of psychiatry.

[46]  Sean A Spence,et al.  Discrete neurophysiological correlates in prefrontal cortex during hysterical and feigned disorder of movement , 2000, The Lancet.

[47]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Imaging hypnotic paralysis: implications for conversion hysteria , 2000, The Lancet.

[48]  Dennis Velakoulis,et al.  The voluntary control of motor imagery. Imagined movements in individuals with feigned motor impairment and conversion disorder , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[50]  R. Ferrari,et al.  Symptom expectation after minor head injury. A comparative study between Canada and Lithuania , 2001, Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery.

[51]  J. Pratt,et al.  Symbolic Control of Visual Attention , 2001, Psychological science.

[52]  G. Morris,et al.  Psychological Treatment of Nonepileptic Events , 2001, Epilepsy & Behavior.

[53]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  Functional neuroanatomical correlates of hysterical sensorimotor loss. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[54]  Michael Sharpe,et al.  What should we say to patients with symptoms unexplained by disease? The “number needed to offend” , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[55]  K. Roelofs,et al.  Childhood abuse in patients with conversion disorder. , 2002, The American journal of psychiatry.

[56]  C. Frith,et al.  Experiencing Oneself vs Another Person as Being the Cause of an Action: The Neural Correlates of the Experience of Agency , 2002, NeuroImage.

[57]  C. L. Kwan,et al.  Altered central somatosensory processing in chronic pain patients with “hysterical” anesthesia , 2003, Neurology.

[58]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Action prediction in the cerebellum and in the parietal lobe , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[59]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Two Eyes for an Eye: The Neuroscience of Force Escalation , 2003, Science.

[60]  K. Roelofs,et al.  Endogenous and Exogenous Attention in Patients with Conversion Paresis , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[61]  S. Schagen,et al.  A symptom perception approach to common physical symptoms. , 2003, Social science & medicine.

[62]  H. Uemura,et al.  Serum Estradiol Concentration as Measured by HPLC-RIA and Bone Mineral Density in Postmenopausal Women during Hormone Replacement Therapy , 2004, Hormone Research in Paediatrics.

[63]  D. Mumford On the computational architecture of the neocortex , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[64]  M. Trimble,et al.  The syndrome of fixed dystonia: an evaluation of 103 patients. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[65]  R. Passingham,et al.  Attention to Intention , 2004, Science.

[66]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[67]  M. Broome A neuroscience of hysteria? , 2004 .

[68]  Richard J. Brown,et al.  Psychological mechanisms of medically unexplained symptoms: an integrative conceptual model. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[69]  Michael Hauck,et al.  Cortical correlates of false expectations during pain intensity judgments—a possible manifestation of placebo/nocebo cognitions , 2005, Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.

[70]  Karl J. Friston Hallucinations and perceptual inference , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[71]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[72]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Hysterical conversion and brain function. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[73]  Parashkev Nachev,et al.  Volition and Conflict in Human Medial Frontal Cortex , 2005, Current Biology.

[74]  S. Dakin,et al.  Weak suppression of visual context in chronic schizophrenia , 2005, Current Biology.

[75]  R. Buchbinder,et al.  Effects of a Media Campaign on Back Beliefs is Sustained 3 Years After Its Cessation , 2005, Spine.

[76]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Evidence for sensory prediction deficits in schizophrenia. , 2005, The American journal of psychiatry.

[77]  C. Warlow,et al.  Eliot Slater's myth of the non-existence of hysteria. , 2005, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[78]  Karin Roelofs,et al.  Hyperactive action monitoring during motor-initiation in conversion paralysis: An event-related potential study , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[79]  G. Lewis,et al.  Multi-symptom illnesses, unexplained illness and Gulf War Syndrome , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[80]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A free energy principle for the brain , 2006, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[81]  P. Rossini,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus alpha rhythms are related to conscious visual perception: a high-resolution EEG study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[82]  D. Sharpe,et al.  Non-epileptic seizures and child sexual abuse: a critical review of the literature. , 2006, Clinical psychology review.

[83]  Steven Lemm,et al.  Now you feel it--now you don't: ERP correlates of somatosensory awareness. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[84]  A. David,et al.  Imaging Repressed Memories in Motor Conversion Disorder , 2007, Psychosomatic medicine.

[85]  Enrico Simonotto,et al.  fMRI in Patients With Motor Conversion Symptoms and Controls With Simulated Weakness , 2007, Psychosomatic medicine.

[86]  W. Rief,et al.  Memory bias for health-related information in somatoform disorders. , 2007, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[87]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[88]  Richard J. Brown,et al.  Development of a paradigm for measuring somatic disturbance in clinical populations with medically unexplained symptoms. , 2008, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[89]  Knut Schnell,et al.  Correlation of passivity symptoms and dysfunctional visuomotor action monitoring in psychosis , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[90]  K. Wiech,et al.  Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[91]  Matthias J. Wieser,et al.  Distinct effects of attention and affect on pain perception and somatosensory evoked potentials , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[92]  A. Lang,et al.  Psychogenic movement disorders , 2009, Current opinion in neurology.

[93]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Motor inhibition in hysterical conversion paralysis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[94]  Paul C. Fletcher,et al.  Why Do Delusions Persist? , 2009, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.

[95]  C. Warlow,et al.  Symptoms 'unexplained by organic disease' in 1144 new neurology out-patients: how often does the diagnosis change at follow-up? , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[96]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The mismatch negativity: A review of underlying mechanisms , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[97]  R. Prescott,et al.  The role of physical injury in motor and sensory conversion symptoms: a systematic and narrative review. , 2009, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[98]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Attention, Uncertainty, and Free-Energy , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[99]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Action and behavior: a free-energy formulation , 2010, Biological Cybernetics.

[100]  E. Holmes,et al.  Induction of Depressed Mood Disrupts Emotion Regulation Neurocircuitry and Enhances Pain Unpleasantness , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[101]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Toward a neurobiology of delusions , 2010, Progress in Neurobiology.

[102]  Michael Sharpe,et al.  The symptom of functional weakness: a controlled study of 107 patients. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[103]  C. Warlow,et al.  Who is referred to neurology clinics?—The diagnoses made in 3781 new patients , 2010, Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery.

[104]  P. Halligan,et al.  Dissociation in hysteria and hypnosis: evidence from cognitive neuroscience , 2010, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[105]  M. Parsonage,et al.  The cost of somatisation among the working-age population in England for the year 2008-2009. , 2010, Mental health in family medicine.

[106]  Richard J. Brown,et al.  Illusory touch and tactile perception in somatoform dissociators. , 2010, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[107]  M. Hallett,et al.  Emotional stimuli and motor conversion disorder. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[108]  Caspar M. Schwiedrzik,et al.  Expectations Change the Signatures and Timing of Electrophysiological Correlates of Perceptual Awareness , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[109]  The involuntary nature of conversion disorder , 2011 .

[110]  M. Hallett,et al.  Psychopathology and psychogenic movement disorders , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[111]  M. Edwards,et al.  Immediate response to botulinum toxin injections in patients with fixed dystonia , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[112]  C. Warlow,et al.  Functional weakness: clues to mechanism from the nature of onset , 2011, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[113]  P. Haggard,et al.  Abnormal sense of intention preceding voluntary movement in patients with psychogenic tremor , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[114]  Reinhold Schmidt,et al.  Moving Toward “Laboratory-Supported” Criteria for Psychogenic Tremor , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[115]  C. Warlow,et al.  Which neurological diseases are most likely to be associated with “symptoms unexplained by organic disease” , 2011, Journal of Neurology.

[116]  Kailash P Bhatia,et al.  Believing is perceiving: mismatch between self-report and actigraphy in psychogenic tremor. , 2012, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[117]  I. Parees,et al.  ‘Jumping to conclusions’ bias in functional movement disorders , 2012, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.