An N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor agonist facilitates sleep-independent synaptic plasticity associated with working memory capacity enhancement

Working memory (WM) capacity improvement is impacted by sleep, and possibly by N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) agonists such as D-cycloserine (DCS), which also affects procedural skill performance. However, the mechanisms behind these relationships are not well understood. In order to investigate the neural basis underlying relationships between WM skill learning and sleep, DCS, and both sleep and DCS together, we evaluated training-retest performances in the n-back task among healthy subjects who were given either a placebo or DCS before the task training, and then followed task training sessions either with wakefulness or sleep. DCS facilitated WM capacity enhancement only occurring after a period of wakefulness, rather than sleep, indicating that WM capacity enhancement is affected by a cellular heterogeneity in synaptic plasticity between time spent awake and time spent asleep. These findings may contribute to development, anti-aging processes, and rehabilitation of higher cognition.

[1]  Yaakov Stern,et al.  Hippocampal Atrophy Relates to Fluid Intelligence Decline in the Elderly , 2010, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[2]  M. Corsi-Cabrera,et al.  Time course of reaction time and EEG while performing a vigilance task during total sleep deprivation. , 1996, Sleep.

[3]  B. Moghaddam,et al.  Functional Interaction Between NMDA and mGlu5 Receptors: Effects on Working Memory, Instrumental Learning, Motor Behaviors, and Dopamine Release , 2004, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[4]  Björn Rasch,et al.  Visual–Procedural Memory Consolidation during Sleep Blocked by Glutamatergic Receptor Antagonists , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  F. Scheer,et al.  An Endogenous Circadian Rhythm in Sleep Inertia Results in Greatest Cognitive Impairment upon Awakening during the Biological Night , 2008, Journal of biological rhythms.

[7]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Sleep, Learning, and Dreams: Off-line Memory Reprocessing , 2001, Science.

[8]  G. Barker,et al.  NMDA Receptor Plasticity in the Perirhinal and Prefrontal Cortices Is Crucial for the Acquisition of Long-Term Object-in-Place Associative Memory , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  E. Vogel,et al.  Quantity, not quality: the relationship between fluid intelligence and working memory capacity , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  A. Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory capacity , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[11]  K D Miller,et al.  Visual responses in adult cat visual cortex depend on N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors. , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  Emotional working memory capacity in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) , 2011, Behaviour research and therapy.

[13]  T. Klingberg Concurrent performance of two working memory tasks: potential mechanisms of interference. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  J. Doyon,et al.  Effect of amantadine on motor memory consolidation in humans , 2005, Behavioural pharmacology.

[15]  Carlyle T. Smith,et al.  Sleep states and memory processes , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[16]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[17]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Sleep-dependent learning and motor-skill complexity. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[18]  Tyrone D. Cannon,et al.  Dysbindin Modulates Prefrontal Cortical Glutamatergic Circuits and Working Memory Function in Mice , 2009, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[19]  M. Bear,et al.  Experience-dependent modification of synaptic plasticity in visual cortex , 1996, Nature.

[20]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Sleep-Dependent Learning and Memory Consolidation , 2004, Neuron.

[21]  Charli Spier Deprivation. , 2019, Health visitor.

[22]  S. Hale,et al.  Processing Speed, Working Memory, and Fluid Intelligence: Evidence for a Developmental Cascade , 1996 .

[23]  B. Schmeichel,et al.  Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  Verbal and Spatial Working Memory in School-Age Children: Developmental Differences in Susceptibility to Interference. , 1997 .

[25]  T. Sawaguchi,et al.  Similar prefrontal cortical activities between general fluid intelligence and visuospatial working memory tasks in preschool children as revealed by optical topography , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Practice with Sleep Makes Perfect Sleep-Dependent Motor Skill Learning , 2002, Neuron.

[27]  M. G. Mulinos,et al.  Absorption, distribution, and excretion of cycloserine in man. , 1955, Antibiotics annual.

[28]  I. G. Campbell,et al.  Sleep deprivation impairs long-term potentiation in rat hippocampal slices. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Yoshiharu Kim,et al.  Effect of d-cycloserine and valproic acid on the extinction of reinstated fear-conditioned responses and habituation of fear conditioning in healthy humans: a randomized controlled trial , 2011, Psychopharmacology.

[30]  Michael Davis,et al.  Facilitation of Conditioned Fear Extinction by Systemic Administration or Intra-Amygdala Infusions of d-Cycloserine as Assessed with Fear-Potentiated Startle in Rats , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Barbara O Rothbaum,et al.  Cognitive enhancers as adjuncts to psychotherapy: use of D-cycloserine in phobic individuals to facilitate extinction of fear. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[32]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  From sounds to words: A neurocomputational model of adaptation, inhibition and memory processes in auditory change detection , 2011, NeuroImage.

[33]  F. Gomez-Pinilla,et al.  Suppression of hippocampal plasticity‐related gene expression by sleep deprivation in rats , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[34]  S. Rumpel,et al.  Silent Synapses in the Developing Rat Visual Cortex: Evidence for Postsynaptic Expression of Synaptic Plasticity , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  S Hale,et al.  Verbal and spatial working memory in school-age children: developmental differences in susceptibility to interference. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[36]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis , 2008, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[37]  R. McCarley,et al.  Hippocampal synaptic plasticity and spatial learning are impaired in a rat model of sleep fragmentation , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[38]  J. Hobson,et al.  Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  A Gevins,et al.  Dynamic cortical networks of verbal and spatial working memory: effects of memory load and task practice. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Behavior-dependent short-term assembly dynamics in the medial prefrontal cortex , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  B. Everitt,et al.  Reconsolidation and Extinction of Conditioned Fear: Inhibition and Potentiation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  R. Coppola,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Brain Mapping in Psychiatry: Methodological Issues Illustrated in a Study of Working Memory in Schizophrenia , 1998, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[43]  A. Karni,et al.  Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. , 1994, Science.

[44]  Yoshiharu Kim,et al.  d-cycloserine facilitates procedural learning but not declarative learning in healthy humans: A randomized controlled trial of the effect of d-cycloserine and valproic acid on overnight properties in the performance of non-emotional memory tasks , 2011, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[45]  W. Singer,et al.  Long-term potentiation and NMDA receptors in rat visual cortex , 1987, Nature.

[46]  J. Seamans,et al.  Synaptic basis of persistent activity in prefrontal cortex in vivo and in organotypic cultures. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  R. Coppola,et al.  Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[48]  A. Gevins,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working memory. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[49]  M. Bouton,et al.  D-cycloserine facilitates extinction but does not eliminate renewal of the conditioned emotional response. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.

[50]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation , 2003, Nature.

[51]  P. Best,et al.  The Effects of MK-801 on Spatial Working Memory and Within-Session Spatial Learning , 1998, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[52]  J. Born,et al.  The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[53]  Tyrone D. Cannon,et al.  Reduced Dysbindin Expression Mediates N-Methyl-D-Aspartate Receptor Hypofunction and Impaired Working Memory Performance , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[54]  Roberto Colom,et al.  General intelligence and memory span: Evidence for a common neuroanatomic framework , 2007, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[55]  G. Collingridge,et al.  Frequency-dependent involvement of NMDA receptors in the hippocampus: a novel synaptic mechanism , 1986, Nature.

[56]  S. Rauch,et al.  Augmentation of behavior therapy with D-cycloserine for obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2008, The American journal of psychiatry.

[57]  P. Goldman-Rakic The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[58]  A. Sanabria,et al.  Randomized controlled trial. , 2005, World journal of surgery.

[59]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Changes in cortical activity after training of working memory — a single-subject analysis , 2007, Physiology & Behavior.

[61]  Min Zhuo,et al.  Roles of NMDA NR2B Subtype Receptor in Prefrontal Long-Term Potentiation and Contextual Fear Memory , 2005, Neuron.

[62]  Christopher B. Donahue,et al.  D-Cycloserine Augmented Exposure Therapy for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[63]  E. Grzęda,et al.  Differentiations of the effect of NMDA on the spatial learning of rats with 4 and 12 week diabetes mellitus. , 2008, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[64]  G. Einstein,et al.  Examining the impact of integrating physical activity on fluid intelligence and academic performance in an elementary school setting: a preliminary investigation. , 2010, Journal of physical activity & health.

[65]  John Duncan,et al.  Fluid intelligence loss linked to restricted regions of damage within frontal and parietal cortex , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[66]  K. Kendrick,et al.  The N-Methyl-D-Aspartate Receptor Co-agonist D-Cycloserine Facilitates Declarative Learning and Hippocampal Activity in Humans , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[67]  Sunbin Song,et al.  Sleep Does Not Benefit Probabilistic Motor Sequence Learning , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[68]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control access to working memory , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[69]  Patrick C. Kyllonen,et al.  Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! , 1990 .

[70]  Phillip J. Holcomb,et al.  Mechanisms Underlying Age- and Performance-related Differences in Working Memory , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[71]  Makoto Uchiyama,et al.  Sleep Accelerates the Improvement in Working Memory Performance , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.