Inflectional frames in language production

The authors report six implicit priming experiments that examined the production of inflected forms. Participants produced words out of small sets in response to prompts. The words differed in form or shared word-initial segments, which allowed for preparation. In constant inflectional sets, the words had the same number of inflectional suffixes, whereas in variable sets the number of suffixes differed. In the experiments, preparation effects were obtained, which were larger in the constant than in the variable sets. Control experiments showed that this difference in effect was not due to syntactic class or phonological form per se. The results are interpreted in terms of a slot-and-filler model of word production, in which inflectional frames, on the one hand, and stems and affixes, on the other hand, are independently spelled out on the basis of an abstract morpho-syntactic specification of the word, which is followed by morpheme-to-frame association.

[1]  H. H. Clark The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. , 1973 .

[2]  M. F. Garrett,et al.  The Analysis of Sentence Production1 , 1975 .

[3]  K. Forster,et al.  More on the language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: Monte Carlo estimates of error rates for F1,F2,F′, and min F′ , 1976 .

[4]  John L. Santa,et al.  Using Quasi F to prevent alpha inflation due to stimulus variation. , 1979 .

[5]  M. Garrett Levels of processing in sentence production , 1980 .

[6]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[7]  Scott E. Maxwell,et al.  Robustness of the Quasi F statistic to violations of sphericity. , 1986 .

[8]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[9]  A. Baddeley Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition , 1990 .

[10]  A. Meyer The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word ☆ , 1990 .

[11]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition , 1990 .

[12]  A. Meyer The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable , 1991 .

[13]  Pienie Zwitserlood,et al.  The role of semantic transparency in the processing and representation of Dutch compounds , 1994 .

[14]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. , 1994 .

[15]  A. Roelofs,et al.  Serial Order in Planning the Production of Successive Morphemes of a Word , 1996 .

[16]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[17]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Morpheme frequency in speech production: Testing weaver , 1997 .

[18]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Syllabification in Speech Production: Evaluation of WEAVER , 1997 .

[19]  A. Roelofs,et al.  The WEAVER model of word-form encoding in speech production , 1997, Cognition.

[20]  Kim Plunkett,et al.  Language Acquisition and Connectionism. , 1998 .

[21]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Morphology in word recognition , 1998 .

[22]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Rightward incrementality in encoding simple phrasal forms in speech production : Verb-particle combinations , 1998 .

[23]  A. Roelofs,et al.  Metrical structure in planning the production of spoken words , 1998 .

[24]  J. Raaijmakers,et al.  How to deal with "The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy": Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. , 1999 .

[25]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Phonological Segments and Features as Planning Units in Speech Production , 1999 .

[26]  Robert A. P. Reuter,et al.  Lexical Entries and Rules of Language : A Multidisciplinary Study of German Inflection , 1999 .

[27]  H. Clahsen,et al.  Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[28]  The place of analogy in Minimalist Morphology and the irregularity of regular forms , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.