Gestural Control in the English Past-Tense Suffix: An Articulatory Study Using Real-Time MRI

The English past tense allomorph following a coronal stop (e.g., /bɑndəd/) includes a vocoid that has traditionally been transcribed as a schwa or as a barred i. Previous evidence has suggested that this entity does not involve a specific articulatory gesture of any kind. Rather, its presence may simply result from temporal coordination of the two temporally adjacent coronal gestures, while the interval between those two gestures remains voiced and is acoustically reminiscent of a schwa. The acoustic and articulatory characteristics of this vocoid are reexamined in this work using real-time MRI with synchronized audio which affords complete midsagittal views of the vocal tract. A novel statistical analysis is developed to address the issue of articulatory targetlessness based on previous models that predict articulatory action from segmental context. Results reinforce the idea that this vocoid is different, both acoustically and articulatorily, than lexical schwa, but its targetless nature is not supported. Data suggest that an articulatory target does exist, especially in the pharynx where it is revealed by the new data acquisition methodology. Moreover, substantial articulatory differences are observed between subjects, which highlights both the difficulty in characterizing this entity previously, and the need for further study with additional subjects.

[1]  M Stone,et al.  Comparison of speech production in upright and supine position. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  Shrikanth S. Narayanan,et al.  An investigation of articulatory setting using real-time magnetic resonance imaging. , 2013, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  Elizabeth C. Zsiga ARTICULATORY TIMING IN A SECOND LANGUAGE , 2003, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[4]  Alan Wrench,et al.  An Ultrasound Protocol for Comparing Tongue Contours: Upright vs Supine , 2011, ICPhS.

[5]  Shinobu Masaki,et al.  Difference in vocal tract shape between upright and supine postures: Observations by an open-type MRI scanner , 2005 .

[6]  Louis Goldstein,et al.  Gesture, Segment, Prosody: “Targetless” schwa: an articulatory analysis , 1992 .

[7]  S. Pinker,et al.  On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition , 1988, Cognition.

[8]  Mohsen Pourahmadi pourahm Construction of Skew-Normal Random Variables : Are They Linear Combinations of Normal and Half-Normal ? , 2009 .

[9]  Edward Flemming,et al.  Rosa's roses: reduced vowels in American English , 2007, Journal of the International Phonetic Association.

[10]  R. Feise Do multiple outcome measures require p-value adjustment? , 2002, BMC medical research methodology.

[11]  Shrikanth S. Narayanan,et al.  Data-driven analysis of realtime vocal tract MRI using correlated image regions , 2010, INTERSPEECH.

[12]  C. Gunnar,et al.  Transmission Properties of the Vocal Tract—A Study Based on Analysis of Swedish Vowels and Consonants , 1951 .

[13]  Iris Smorodinsky Schwas with and without active gestural control , 2001 .

[14]  Line Mikkelsen,et al.  Representing Language: Essays in Honor of Judith Aissen , 2011 .

[15]  K J Rothman,et al.  No Adjustments Are Needed for Multiple Comparisons , 1990, Epidemiology.

[16]  Adamantios I. Gafos,et al.  A Grammar of Gestural Coordination , 2002 .

[17]  T. Perneger What's wrong with Bonferroni adjustments , 1998, BMJ.

[18]  Josef Kittler,et al.  Pattern recognition : a statistical approach , 1982 .

[19]  Edward Flemming The Phonetics of Schwa Vowels , 2009 .

[20]  Eric Baković,et al.  Antigemination, assimilation and the determination of identity , 2005, Phonology.

[21]  C. Browman,et al.  Competing constraints on intergestural coordination and self-organization of phonological structures , 2000 .

[22]  Michael B. Miller,et al.  of Serendipitous and Unexpected Results Neural Correlates of Interspecies Perspective Taking in the Post-Mortem Atlantic Salmon : An Argument For Proper Multiple Comparisons Correction , 2010 .

[23]  Bruce Hayes,et al.  Linguistics: An Introduction to Linguistic Theory , 2001 .

[24]  CM Bennett,et al.  Neural correlates of interspecies perspective taking in the post-mortem Atlantic Salmon: an argument for multiple comparisons correction , 2009, NeuroImage.

[25]  Lisa Davidson,et al.  Phonotactics and Articulatory Coordination Interact in Phonology: Evidence from Nonnative Production , 2006, Cogn. Sci..

[26]  Shrikanth S. Narayanan,et al.  Region Segmentation in the Frequency Domain Applied to Upper Airway Real-Time Magnetic Resonance Images , 2009, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[27]  Shrikanth Narayanan,et al.  Synchronized and noise-robust audio recordings during realtime magnetic resonance imaging scans. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  Dani Byrd,et al.  Action to Language via the Mirror Neuron System: The role of vocal tract gestural action units in understanding the evolution of phonology , 2006 .

[29]  Jean Ponce,et al.  Computer Vision: A Modern Approach , 2002 .

[30]  Shrikanth Narayanan,et al.  An approach to real-time magnetic resonance imaging for speech production. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer , 2002 .

[32]  Paul Kiparsky,et al.  Some consequences of Lexical Phonology , 1985, Phonology Yearbook.

[33]  Richard L. Smith,et al.  PREDICTIVE INFERENCE , 2004 .