The hazards of explanation: overgeneralization in the face of exceptions.

Seeking explanations is central to science, education, and everyday thinking, and prompting learners to explain is often beneficial. Nonetheless, in 2 category learning experiments across artifact and social domains, we demonstrate that the very properties of explanation that support learning can impair learning by fostering overgeneralizations. We find that explaining encourages learners to seek broad patterns, hindering learning when patterns involve exceptions. By revealing how effects of explanation depend on the structure of what is being learned, these experiments simultaneously demonstrate the hazards of explaining and provide evidence for why explaining is so often beneficial. For better or for worse, explaining recruits the remarkable human capacity to seek underlying patterns that go beyond individual observations.

[1]  T. Kuhn,et al.  The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. , 1964 .

[2]  M. Friedman Explanation and Scientific Understanding , 1974 .

[3]  P. Kitcher Explanatory Unification , 1981, Philosophy of Science.

[4]  R. Mathews,et al.  Role of Implicit and Explicit Processes in Learning From Examples: A Synergistic Effect , 1989 .

[5]  P. Kitcher Explanatory unification and the causal structure of the world , 1989 .

[6]  I. Begg,et al.  Problem-oriented training promotes spontaneous analogical transfer: Memory-oriented training promotes memory for training , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[7]  D Broadbent,et al.  Implicit and explicit knowledge bases in artificial grammar learning. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  A. Reber Implicit learning and tacit knowledge , 1993 .

[9]  William F. Brewer,et al.  The Role of Anomalous Data in Knowledge Acquisition: A Theoretical Framework and Implications for Science Instruction , 1993 .

[10]  D. Shanks,et al.  Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[11]  E M Reingold,et al.  Toward a redefinition of implicit memory: process dissociations following elaborative processing and self-generation. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Eliciting Self-Explanations Improves Understanding , 1994, Cogn. Sci..

[13]  G. Murphy,et al.  Category learning with minimal prior knowledge. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  C. Meissner,et al.  Verbal overshadowing: A special issue exploring theoretical and applied issues , 2002 .

[15]  Jonathan W. Schooler,et al.  Verbalization produces a transfer inappropriate processing shift. , 2002 .

[16]  R. Siegler Microgenetic Studies of Self-Explanation , 2002 .

[17]  B. Love Comparing supervised and unsupervised category learning , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[18]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Fast and frugal heuristics: The tools of bounded rationality , 2004 .

[19]  R. Mathews,et al.  Role of Implicit and Explicit Processes in Learning From Examples : A Synergistic Effect , 2004 .

[20]  David L. Faigman,et al.  Human category learning. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.

[21]  H. Wellman,et al.  Theories of Mind in Transition: A Microgenetic Study of the Development of False Belief Understanding , 2006 .

[22]  T. Lombrozo The structure and function of explanations , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Promoting transfer: effects of self-explanation and direct instruction. , 2006, Child development.

[24]  Jane L. Risen,et al.  One-Shot Illusory Correlations and Stereotype Formation , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[25]  H. Wellman,et al.  Causal Reasoning as Informed by the Early Development of Explanations , 2007 .

[26]  C. Frith Social cognition , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  I. Brocas,et al.  Theories of the Mind , 2008 .

[28]  D. Kuhn,et al.  Are self-explanations always beneficial? , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  Percival G. Matthews,et al.  In pursuit of knowledge: comparing self-explanations, concepts, and procedures as pedagogical tools. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[30]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Active-Constructive-Interactive: A Conceptual Framework for Differentiating Learning Activities , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[31]  H. Wellman,et al.  Inconsistency with prior knowledge triggers children's causal explanatory reasoning. , 2010, Child development.

[32]  Joseph Jay Williams,et al.  The role of explanation in discovery and generalization: evidence from category learning , 2010, ICLS.

[33]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Bootstrapping the Mind: Analogical Processes and Symbol Systems , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[34]  J. J. Williams,et al.  Why does explaining help learning? Insight from an explanation impairment effect , 2010 .

[35]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  The double-edged effects of explanation prompts , 2011, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[36]  Joseph Jay Williams,et al.  Explaining drives the discovery of real and illusory patterns , 2011, CogSci.

[37]  B. Malle Attribution theories: How people make sense of behavior. , 2011 .

[38]  T. Lombrozo Explanation and Abductive Inference , 2012 .

[39]  E. Markman,et al.  Thinking in categories or along a continuum: consequences for children's social judgments. , 2012, Child development.

[40]  J. J. Williams,et al.  Explanation and prior knowledge interact to guide learning , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.

[41]  Shelia M. Kennison,et al.  Theories of Mind , 2013 .