Intentional Switching Between Bimanual Coordination Patterns

ABSTRACT Previous theoretical and empirical work indicates that intentional changes in a bimanual coordination pattern depends on the stability of the bimanual coordination pattern (Kelso, Schotz, & Schöner, 1988; Scholz & Kelso, 1990). The present experiments retest this notion when online Lissajous displays are provided. Switching to and from in-phase and antiphase and to and from 90° and 270° were tested in Experiment 1. Participants were able to very effectively produce the 180°, 90°, and 270° coordination patterns although performance of the in-phase coordination task was even more stable. The data indicated that switching to in-phase from antiphase was more rapid than vice versa and that switching times between 90° to 270° were similar. Experiment 2 investigated switching between 1:2 and 2:1 bimanual coordination patterns. The results indicated that switching time was similar between the 2:1 and 1:2 coordination tasks and that increases in stability over practice resulted in additional decreases in switching times. This provides additional evidence that the attractor landscape is fundamentally different dependent on the type of information provided the performer. What remains to be done is to reconcile these results with the various theories/perspectives currently used to describe and explain bimanual coordination.

[1]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Distinguishing between the effects of frequency and amplitude on interlimb coupling in tapping a 2:3 polyrhythm , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Bimanual 1:1 with 90° continuous relative phase: difficult or easy! , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Symmetrical and asymmetrical influences on force production in 1:2 and 2:1 bimanual force coordination tasks , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  J. Kelso,et al.  Governing coordination: behavioural principles and neural correlates , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  Bruce Abernethy,et al.  The contribution of inherent and incidental constraints to intentional switching between patterns of bimanual coordination , 1996 .

[6]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands , 1980, Biological Cybernetics.

[8]  Chaoyi Wang,et al.  Bimanual force control: cooperation and interference? , 2016, Psychological research.

[9]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Resonance Constraints on Rhythmic Movement , 1993 .

[10]  O Bock,et al.  Learning of a new bimanual coordination pattern is governed by three distinct processes. , 2001, Motor control.

[11]  Charles H Shea,et al.  Perceptual and attentional influences on continuous 2:1 and 3:2 multi-frequency bimanual coordination. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  David R. Collins,et al.  Perceptual coupling in rhythmic movement coordination: stable perception leads to stable action , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  S. Cardoso de Oliveira The neuronal basis of bimanual coordination: recent neurophysiological evidence and functional models. , 2002, Acta psychologica.

[14]  Angelika Foerster,et al.  Coordination Neural Behavioral And Social Dynamics , 2016 .

[15]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Amplitude differences, spatial assimilation, and integrated feedback in bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  Chapter 18 Another timing variable composed of state variables: Phase perception and phase driven oscillators , 2004 .

[17]  G P Bingham,et al.  Visual perception of the relative phasing of human limb movements , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  P. Beek,et al.  Intentional switches between bimanual coordination patterns are primarily effectuated by the nondominant hand. , 2006, Motor control.

[19]  H. Haken,et al.  A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand movement , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[20]  C Lieke E Peper,et al.  Bimanual training for children with cerebral palsy: Exploring the effects of Lissajous-based computer gaming , 2013, Developmental neurorehabilitation.

[21]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Dynamical Models of Movement Coordination , 1995 .

[22]  Y. Guiard On Fitts's and Hooke's laws: simple harmonic movement in upper-limb cyclical aiming. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[23]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Unraveling interlimb interactions underlying bimanual coordination. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  The learning of 90° continuous relative phase with and without Lissajous feedback: external and internally generated bimanual coordination. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[25]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Intentional switching between patterns of coordination in bimanual circle drawing , 1998 .

[26]  P G Zanone,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Interlimb coordination: Learning and transfer under different feedback conditions , 1997 .

[28]  J. S. Scott Kelso,et al.  Haptic information stabilizes and destabilizes coordination dynamics , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  J. Kelso,et al.  Dynamics governs switching among patterns of coordination in biological movement , 1988 .

[30]  John J. Buchanan,et al.  Learning an environment–actor coordination skill: visuomotor transformation and coherency of perceptual structure , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  R. Carson Neural pathways mediating bilateral interactions between the upper limbs , 2005, Brain Research Reviews.

[32]  John J Buchanan Perceptual Estimates of Motor Skill Proficiency Are Constrained by the Stability of Coordination Patterns , 2015, Journal of motor behavior.

[33]  P. V. Wieringen,et al.  Coupling strength in tapping a 2:3 polyrhythm , 1995 .

[34]  W. Prinz,et al.  Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination , 2001, Nature.

[35]  Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al.  A Perceptually Driven Dynamical Model of Bimanual Rhythmic Movement (and Phase Perception) , 2004 .

[36]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Using scanning trials to assess intrinsic coordination dynamics , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[37]  Stefan Panzer,et al.  Continuous scanning trials:Transitioning through the attractor landscape , 2016, Neuroscience Letters.

[38]  John J Buchanan,et al.  The Interaction of Tactile Information and Movement Amplitude in a Multijoint Bimanual Circle-Tracing Task: Phase Transitions and Loss of Stability , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[39]  Paul Van Hecke,et al.  Internal vs external generation of movements: differential neural pathways involved in bimanual coordination performed in the presence or absence of augmented visual feedback , 2003, NeuroImage.

[40]  Pier-Giorgio Zanone,et al.  Evaluating the coordination dynamics of handwriting. , 2004, Human movement science.

[41]  Gavan Lintern,et al.  Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior , 1997, Complex.

[42]  R. Ivry,et al.  A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective on Bimanual Coordination and Interference , 2004 .

[43]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Frequency-induced phase transitions in bimanual tapping , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[44]  Hermann Haken Synergetik: Nichtgleichgewichte, Phasenübergänge und Selbstorganisation , 2005, Naturwissenschaften.

[45]  K. Zilles,et al.  The role of ventral medial wall motor areas in bimanual co-ordination. A combined lesion and activation study. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  J. Kelso,et al.  Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: collective and component levels. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Error correction in bimanual coordination benefits from bilateral muscle activity: evidence from kinesthetic tracking , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  Nina Robson,et al.  The perception–action dynamics of action competency are altered by both physical and observational training , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  Paul Van Hecke,et al.  Brain Areas Involved in Interlimb Coordination: A Distributed Network , 2001, NeuroImage.

[50]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  A model for phase transitions in human hand movements during multifrequency tapping , 1996 .

[51]  J. Kelso,et al.  Environmentally-specified patterns of movement coordination in normal and split-brain subjects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Relative Phase Alterations During Bimanual Skill Acquisition. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[53]  R. Carson,et al.  The acquisition of movement skills: practice enhances the dynamic stability of bimanual coordination. , 2001, Human movement science.

[54]  P. Cavallari,et al.  Differential control of in-phase and anti-phase coupling of rhythmic movements of ipsilateral hand and foot , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  T D Lee,et al.  Learning a new bimanual coordination pattern: reciprocal influences of intrinsic and to-be-learned patterns. , 1997, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[56]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Two hands, one brain: cognitive neuroscience of bimanual skill , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[57]  D J Serrien,et al.  Intentional switching between behavioral patterns of homologous and nonhomologous effector combinations. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[58]  A. J. Kovacs,et al.  Perceptual influences on Fitts’ law , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[59]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Impossible is nothing: 5:3 and 4:3 multi-frequency bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[60]  J. Kelso,et al.  Intentional switching between patterns of bimanual coordination depends on the intrinsic dynamics of the patterns. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[61]  J. Kelso,et al.  Multifrequency behavioral patterns and the phase attractive circle map , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.

[62]  J. Kelso,et al.  Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations , 1986 .

[63]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .

[64]  Andrew D. Wilson,et al.  Human movement coordination implicates relative direction as the information for relative phase , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[65]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[66]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Rhythmical bimanual force production: homologous and non-homologous muscles , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[67]  Natalia Dounskaia,et al.  Preferred and induced coordination modes during the acquisition of bimanual movements with a 2 :1 frequency ratio , 1997 .

[68]  Charles H Shea,et al.  Perception and action influences on discrete and reciprocal bimanual coordination , 2015, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[69]  J. A. S. Kelso,et al.  Order in Time: How the Cooperation Between the Hands Informs the Design of the Brain , 1988 .

[70]  Silvia Comani,et al.  Striatal Activity during Intentional Switching Depends on Pattern Stability , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[71]  Pier-Giorgio Zanone,et al.  Switching among graphic patterns is governed by oscillatory coordination dynamics: implications for understanding handwriting , 2013, Front. Psychol..