Common Priority Map for Selection History, Reward and Emotion in the Oculomotor System
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[2] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[3] D. Sparks,et al. The deep layers of the superior colliculus. , 1989, Reviews of oculomotor research.
[4] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[5] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[6] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[7] H. Deubel,et al. Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.
[8] R. Wurtz,et al. Reversible inactivation of monkey superior colliculus. I. Curvature of saccadic trajectory. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[9] R. Wurtz,et al. Reversible inactivation of monkey superior colliculus. II. Maps of saccadic deficits. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[10] D. E. Irwin,et al. Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .
[11] S. Paradiso. The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life , 1998 .
[12] K. Berridge,et al. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? , 1998, Brain Research Reviews.
[13] R. Walker,et al. A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[14] Michael L. Platt,et al. Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.
[15] Robert M. McPeek,et al. Saccades require focal attention and are facilitated by a short-term memory system , 1999, Vision Research.
[16] N. Cohen,et al. Amnesia is a Deficit in Relational Memory , 2000, Psychological science.
[17] P. Lang,et al. Fear and anxiety: animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. , 2000, Journal of affective disorders.
[18] Robert M. McPeek,et al. Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search , 2000, Vision Research.
[19] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of popout: III. A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection , 2000 .
[20] R. Klein,et al. A Model of Saccade Initiation Based on the Competitive Integration of Exogenous and Endogenous Signals in the Superior Colliculus , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[21] S. Mineka,et al. Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.
[22] R. Walker,et al. Curved saccade trajectories: Voluntary and reflexive saccades curve away from irrelevant distractors , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[23] J. Theeuwes,et al. Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] N. P. Bichot,et al. Priming in Macaque Frontal Cortex during Popout Visual Search: Feature-Based Facilitation and Location-Based Inhibition of Return , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[25] O. Hikosaka,et al. Influence of reward expectation on visuospatial processing in macaque lateral prefrontal cortex. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.
[26] Okihide Hikosaka,et al. Reward-Dependent Gain and Bias of Visual Responses in Primate Superior Colliculus , 2003, Neuron.
[27] Robert M McPeek,et al. Competition between saccade goals in the superior colliculus produces saccade curvature. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[28] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Spatial attention and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[29] T. Moore,et al. Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[30] Ralf Engbert,et al. Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention , 2005, Vision Research.
[31] P. Vuilleumier,et al. How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[32] Arthur F. Kramer,et al. Error-processing of oculomotor capture , 2006, Brain Research.
[33] P. Haggard,et al. Time course of oculomotor inhibition revealed by saccade trajectory modulation. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[34] P. Subramanian. Active Vision: The Psychology of Looking and Seeing , 2006 .
[35] Dominique Lamy,et al. Priming of Pop-out provides reliable measures of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention , 2008, Vision Research.
[36] Jan Theeuwes,et al. The Role of Awareness in Processing of Oculomotor Capture: Evidence from Event-related Potentials , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[37] Stefanie I. Becker,et al. The mechanism of priming: episodic retrieval or priming of pop-out? , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[38] L. Pessoa. On the relationship between emotion and cognition , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[39] Jan Theeuwes,et al. When Are Attention and Saccade Preparation Dissociated? , 2009, Psychological science.
[40] J. Hyönä,et al. Emotional scene content drives the saccade generation system reflexively. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] Deborah E. Hannula,et al. The Eyes Have It: Hippocampal Activity Predicts Expression of Memory in Eye Movements , 2009, Neuron.
[42] G. Campana,et al. Where perception meets memory: A review of repetition priming in visual search tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[43] J. Theeuwes,et al. Evidence for a dissociation between the control of oculomotor capture and disengagement , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[44] L. Pessoa,et al. Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a 'low road' to 'many roads' of evaluating biological significance , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[45] Patryk A. Laurent,et al. Value-driven attentional capture , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[46] J. Theeuwes,et al. The presence of threat affects saccade trajectories , 2012 .
[47] Kalina Petrova,et al. Upper–lower visual field asymmetries in oculomotor inhibition of emotional distractors , 2012, Vision Research.
[48] J. Theeuwes,et al. Updating the premotor theory: the allocation of attention is not always accompanied by saccade preparation. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[49] Steven Yantis,et al. Value-driven attentional and oculomotor capture during goal-directed, unconstrained viewing , 2012, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[50] J. Theeuwes,et al. Lateral interactions in the superior colliculus produce saccade deviation in a neural field model , 2012, Vision Research.
[51] J. Theeuwes,et al. Reward grabs the eye: Oculomotor capture by rewarding stimuli , 2012, Vision Research.
[52] J. Theeuwes,et al. Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[53] W. Zoest,et al. Reward creates oculomotor salience , 2012, Current Biology.
[54] G. Crombez,et al. Conditioned fear modulates visual selection. , 2013, Emotion.
[55] Stefan Van der Stigchel,et al. Saccades curve away from previously inhibited locations: evidence for the role of priming in oculomotor competition. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[56] B. Anderson. A value-driven mechanism of attentional selection. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[57] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Attentional capture by signals of threat , 2014, Cognition & emotion.
[58] J. Theeuwes,et al. Potential threat attracts attention and interferes with voluntary saccades. , 2015, Emotion.
[59] J. Theeuwes,et al. Distractors that signal reward attract the eyes , 2015 .
[60] Tom Beesley,et al. When goals conflict with values: counterproductive attentional and oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.