Human development of perceptual organization

[1]  J. L. Conel,et al.  The postnatal development of the human cerebral cortex , 1960 .

[2]  D. Weintraub,et al.  Ebbinghaus illusion: context, contour, and age influence the judged size of a circle amidst circles. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  A. Fiorentini,et al.  Perceptual learning specific for orientation and spatial frequency , 1980, Nature.

[4]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Cortical binocularity in infants , 1980, Nature.

[5]  David M. Regal,et al.  Development of critical flicker frequency in human infants , 1981, Vision Research.

[6]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[7]  T. Wiesel The postnatal development of the visual cortex and the influence of environment. , 1982, Bioscience reports.

[8]  J. Lund,et al.  Widespread periodic intrinsic connections in the tree shrew visual cortex. , 1982, Science.

[9]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[10]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Clustered intrinsic connections in cat visual cortex , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  E. Spelke,et al.  Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  R. von der Heydt,et al.  Illusory contours and cortical neuron responses. , 1984, Science.

[13]  Marc H. Bornstein,et al.  Human infant color vision and color perception , 1985 .

[14]  D. Teller,et al.  ASSESSMENT OF VISUAL ACUITY IN INFANTS AND CHILDREN; THE ACUITY CARD PROCEDURE , 1986, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[15]  DH Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, and stereopsis in primate area 18 , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  W. Overman Performance on Traditional Matching to Sample, Non‐Matching to Sample, and Object Discrimination Tasks by 12‐ to 32‐Month‐Old Children , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[17]  R. Desimone,et al.  A neural mechanism for working and recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex. , 1991, Science.

[18]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[19]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Visual segmentation of oriented textures by infants , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  Ruxandra Sireteanu,et al.  Texture segregation in infants and children , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[21]  Manfred Fahle,et al.  The development of vernier acuity in human infants , 1991, Vision Research.

[22]  J. Bachevalier,et al.  Object recognition versus object discrimination: comparison between human infants and infant monkeys. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[23]  T Poggio,et al.  Fast perceptual learning in visual hyperacuity. , 1991, Science.

[24]  U. Polat,et al.  Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.

[25]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.

[26]  A. Burkhalter,et al.  Development of local circuits in human visual cortex , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  I Kovács,et al.  A closed curve is much more than an incomplete one: effect of closure in figure-ground segmentation. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  Michael J. Hawken,et al.  Macaque VI neurons can signal ‘illusory’ contours , 1993, Nature.

[29]  A. Burkhalter Development of forward and feedback connections between areas V1 and V2 of human visual cortex. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  A. Karni,et al.  The time course of learning a visual skill , 1993, Nature.

[31]  M. Goodale Visual pathways supporting perception and action in the primate cerebral cortex , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[32]  U. Polat,et al.  The architecture of perceptual spatial interactions , 1994, Vision Research.

[33]  R. Desimone,et al.  Inferior temporal mechanisms for invariant object recognition. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  A. Diamond,et al.  Young children's performance on a task sensitive to the memory functions of the medial temporal lobe in adults--the delayed nonmatching-to-sample task--reveals problems that are due to non-memory-related task demands. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[35]  B. Julesz,et al.  Perceptual sensitivity maps within globally defined visual shapes , 1994, Nature.

[36]  R. Sireteanu,et al.  Texture segmentation and 'pop-out' in infants and children: the effect of test field size. , 1994, Spatial vision.

[37]  Young children's performance on a task sensitive to the memory functions of the medial temporal lobe in adults--the delayed nonmatching-to-sample task--reveals problems that are due to non-memory-related task demands. , 1994 .

[38]  Ruxandra Sireteanu,et al.  Perceptual learning in visual search: Fast, enduring, but non-specific , 1995, Vision Research.

[39]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[40]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.

[41]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Functional development of the corticocortical pathway for motion analysis in the macaque monkey: a 14C-2-deoxyglucose study. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  E. Ciner,et al.  Stereoacuity Development: 6 Months to 5 Years. A New Tool for Testing and Screening , 1996, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[43]  Figural and Semantic Factors in Change in the Ebbinghaus Illusion across Four Age Groups of Children , 1996, Perceptual and motor skills.

[44]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Spatial integration and cortical dynamics. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  C. Colby,et al.  Spatial representations for action in parietal cortex. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[46]  U. Polat,et al.  Neurophysiological Evidence for Contrast Dependent Long-range Facilitation and Suppression in the Human Visual Cortex , 1996, Vision Research.

[47]  T. S. Lee,et al.  Gestalten of Today: Early Processing of Visual Contours and Surfaces , 1996 .

[48]  R. Andersen,et al.  Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[49]  Late Maturation (Age >5 Years) of Long-Range Spatial Interactions in Humans , 1997 .

[50]  D Regan,et al.  Development of Motion-Defined Figure-Ground Segregation in Preschool and Older Children, Using a Letter-Identification Task , 1997, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[51]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration in strabismic amblyopia: The sufficiency of an explanation based on positional uncertainty , 1997, Vision Research.

[52]  D M Levi,et al.  Development of Vernier Acuity in Childhood , 1997, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[53]  M A Goodale,et al.  Vision for perception and vision for action in the primate brain. , 2007, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[54]  S. Crewther,et al.  Development of a Magnocellular Function in Good and Poor Primary School-Age Readers , 1998, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[55]  A P Batista,et al.  Posterior parietal areas specialized for eye movements (LIP) and reach (PRR) using a common coordinate frame. , 1998, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[56]  W R Shankle,et al.  Evidence for a postnatal doubling of neuron number in the developing human cerebral cortex between 15 months and 6 years. , 1998, Journal of theoretical biology.

[57]  John P. Boyd,et al.  Quantitative microscopic anatomy, illustrated by its potential role in furthering understanding of the processes of structuring the developing human cerebral cortex , 1998, Acta paediatrica Japonica : Overseas edition.

[58]  Jan M. Ruijter,et al.  Visual half-field development in children: detection of motion-defined forms , 1998, Vision Research.

[59]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Separate body- and world-referenced representations of visual space in parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[60]  Bela Julesz,et al.  Medial-point description of shape: a representation for action coding and its psychophysical correlates , 1998, Vision Research.

[61]  Marjorie A.M. Hollants-Gilhuijs,et al.  Visual half-field development in children: Detection of colour-contrast-defined forms , 1998, Vision Research.

[62]  R F Hess,et al.  Spatial-frequency tuning of visual contour integration. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[63]  M. Goodale,et al.  Does a monocularly presented size-contrast illusion influence grip aperture? , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  W R Shankle,et al.  Developmental patterns in the cytoarchitecture of the human cerebral cortex from birth to 6 years examined by correspondence analysis. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[65]  W. Shankle,et al.  Constructing the human cerebral cortex during infancy and childhood: Types and numbers of cortical columns and numbers of neurons in such columns at different age‐points , 1998, Acta paediatrica Japonica : Overseas edition.

[66]  R. Hess,et al.  Contour integration in anisometropic amblyopia , 1998, Vision Research.

[67]  C. Gilbert Adult cortical dynamics. , 1998, Physiological reviews.

[68]  M. Goodale,et al.  Frames of Reference for Perception and Action in the Human Visual System , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[69]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  The Effect of Pictorial Illusion on Prehension and Perception , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  A. Dale,et al.  The Representation of Illusory and Real Contours in Human Cortical Visual Areas Revealed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[71]  D. Field,et al.  Integration of contours: new insights , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[72]  U Polat,et al.  Contour detection threshold: repeatability and learning with 'contour cards'. , 1999, Spatial vision.

[73]  J. Braun On the detection of salient contours. , 1999, Spatial vision.

[74]  fMRI study of human brain areas activated by form coherence: Dorsal or ventral function? , 1999 .

[75]  W R Shankle,et al.  Approximate Doubling of Numbers of Neurons in Postnatal Human Cerebral Cortex and in 35 Specific Cytoarchitectural Areas from Birth to 72 Months , 1999, Pediatric and developmental pathology : the official journal of the Society for Pediatric Pathology and the Paediatric Pathology Society.

[76]  I. Kovács,et al.  Late maturation of visual spatial integration in humans. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[77]  D. Maurer,et al.  Development of spatial and temporal vision during childhood , 1999, Vision Research.

[78]  Walter H. Ehrenstein,et al.  Development of dynamic vision based on motion contrast , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[79]  B. Lia,et al.  Infant temporal contrast sensitivity functions (tCSFs) mature earlier for luminance than for chromatic stimuli: evidence for precocious magnocellular development? , 1999, Vision Research.

[80]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Form and motion coherence: Is there dorsal stream vulnerability in development , 1999 .

[81]  Ilona Kovács,et al.  A new test of contour integration deficits in patients with a history of disrupted binocular experience during visual development , 2000, Vision Research.

[82]  Ilona Kovács,et al.  Perceptual organization, the disorganization syndrome, and context processing in chronic schizophrenia , 2000, Schizophrenia Research.

[83]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The computation of occluded contours in visual agnosia: Evidence for early computation prior to shape binding and figure-ground coding , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.